Model (Hobson)
Episodic memory is also impaired within the dream. Indeed, a dream is not like an episode of life being “replayed”. In one example in which subjects had intensively played the computer game Tetris, there was no episodic memory in subsequent dreams that subjects had indeed played Tetris. In fact, dreams of healthy subjects were indistinguishable from those of profoundly amnesic subjects, who could not remember having played Tetris at all. In contrast, both normal and amnesic subjects often reported perceptual fragments, such as falling blocks on a computer screen, at sleep onset[ 38 ]. While ‘residues’ from waking experience are incorporated in about 50% of dreams[ 39 – 41 ], they do so in new and unrelated contexts, and verified memories for episodes of recent life are only found in about 1.5% of dreams[ 42 ]. Such residual recollections have been interpreted by some to suggest that dreaming may have an active role in forgetting[ 5 , 43 ]. Finally, many have the impression that the network of associations stored in our memory may become looser than in wake[ 44 , 45 ], perhaps favoring creativity, divergent thinking, and problem resolution[ 4 , 46 ].
In summary, dream consciousness is remarkably similar to waking consciousness, though there are several intriguing differences. These include reduced attention and voluntary control, lack in self-awareness, altered reflective thought, occasional hyperemotionality, and impaired memory. Traditionally, dream phenomenology has often been compared to madness or psychosis[ 3 , 11 , 47 ], but in fact the hallucinations, disorientation, and subsequent amnesia of some bizarre dreams may be more akin to the acute confusional state – also known as delirium - which occurs after withdrawal from alcohol and drugs[ 48 ]. However, most dreams are less bizarre, perhaps more similar to mind wandering or stimulus independent thoughts[ 14 , 49 , 50 ]. Waking thoughts jump around and drift into bizarre daydreaming, rumination, and worrying far more than stereotypes of rational linear thinking suggest[ 51 ]. Importantly, individual dreams are highly variable in their phenomenology, and only some conform to the typical monolithic template that is often portrayed. Thus, just like diverse waking experiences, “Not all dreams are created equal” , and future studies should consider different kinds of dreams and their neural correlates separately.
What mechanisms are responsible for regional differences in brain activity between waking and REM sleep, and thus presumably for some of the cognitive differences between waking and dreaming? Single-unit physiology indicates that generally, cortical activity in REM sleep reaches similar levels as found in active wake ( Fig. 2 ), but variability between brain areas remains poorly explored. Regional differences may likely stem from changes in the activity of neuromodulatory systems ( Fig. 2 ). During REM sleep, acetylcholine is alone in maintaining brain activation, whereas monoaminergic systems are silent, an observation that could explain many features of dreams[ 11 ]. For example, consistent with imaging results, cholinergic innervation is stronger in limbic and paralimbic areas than in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex[ 52 ], which may explain why limbic regions are highly active in REM sleep while dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is deactivated ( Fig. 1 ). Dopaminergic modulation may also play a role[ 23 ], since dreaming is decreased by prefrontal leucotomies that cut dopaminergic fibers[ 53 ] and is increased by dopaminergic agonists[ 23 ] ( Table 1 and Fig. 2 ).
A comparison of cortical activity (upper panel) and neuromodulator activity (bottom panel) in wake, early NREM (when sleep pressure is high and dream reports are rare), late NREM (when sleep pressure dissipates, and dream reports are more frequent), and REM sleep (when dreams are most common).
(a) Intracellular studies. The membrane potential of cortical neurons in both wake and REM sleep is depolarized and fluctuates around −63mV and −61mV, respectively [ 77 ]. In REM sleep, whenever phasic events such as rapid eye movements and PGO waves occur (gray arrows, events not shown), neurons increase their firing rates to levels that surpass those found in wake [ 77 , 146 ]. In early NREM sleep, neurons alternate between two distinct states, each lasting tens/hundreds of milliseconds: UP states (red arrow) are associated with depolarization and increased firing, while in DOWN states (blue arrow) the membrane potential is hyperpolarized around −75mV, and neuronal firing fades[ 78 , 147 ]. Intracellular studies focusing specifically on late NREM sleep are not available (N.A.).
(b) Extracellular studies. Spiking of individual neurons in REM sleep reaches similar levels as in active wake. In both wake and REM sleep, neurons exhibit tonic irregular asynchronous activity [ 77 , 148 – 151 ]. Sustained activity in wake and REM sleep can be viewed as a continuous UP state [ 78 ] (red bars). In early NREM sleep, UP states are short and synchronous across neuronal populations, and are frequently interrupted by long DOWN states (blue bars). In late NREM sleep, UP states are longer and less synchronized [ 79 ].
(3) Polysomnography. Waking is characterized by low-amplitude, high-frequency EEG activity (above 7Hz), occasional saccadic eye movements, and elevated muscle tone. In early NREM sleep, high-amplitude slow waves (below 4Hz) dominate the EEG. Neuronal UP (red) and DOWN (blue) states correspond to positive and negative peaks in the surface EEG, respectively [ 79 ]. Eye movements are largely absent and muscle tone is decreased. In late NREM sleep, slow waves are less frequent, while spindles (related to UP states and surface EEG positivity) become more common. Eye movements and muscle tone are largely similar to early NREM sleep [ 152 ]. In REM sleep, theta activity (4–7 Hz) prevails, rapid eye movements occur, and muscle tone is dramatically reduced.
(d) Neuromodulator activity. Subcortical cholinergic modulation is highly active in wake and REM sleep (green arrows) and leads to sustained depolarization in cortical neurons and EEG activation [ 77 ]. Wake is further maintained by activity of monoamines, histamine, and hypocretin/orexin (green arrows). In sleep, monoaminergic systems including norepinephrine and serotonin reduce their activity (pink arrows), and are silent in REM sleep (red arrows). While dopamine levels do not change dramatically across the sleep-wake cycle (asterisks), phasic events and regional profiles may differ[ 153 ].
Data are pooled across different species for illustration purposes. Intracellular cat data adapted with permission from Ref [ 77 ]; extracellular and EEG rat data obtained from V. Vyazovskiy (personal communication).
On the whole, relating typical dreams to the neurophysiology of REM sleep has proven to be a useful starting point for revealing the neural basis of dreaming. However, dream consciousness can not be reduced to brain activity in REM sleep. Indeed, some fundamental questions concerning the relationship between the brain and dreaming linger on. We shall discuss three in turn: i) what determines the level of consciousness during sleep; ii) why the dreamer is disconnected from the environment; and iii) whether dreams are more akin to perception or to imagination.
In principle, studying mental experiences during sleep offers a unique opportunity to explain how changes in brain activity relate to changes in consciousness[ 3 , 54 ]. In fact, if it were not for sleep, when consciousness fades in and out on a regular basis, it might be hard to imagine that consciousness is not a given, but depends somehow on the way our brain is functioning. Traditionally, studies have focused on differences among reports obtained after awakenings from different sleep stages or at different times of night. When REM sleep was initially distinguished from NREM sleep[ 55 ], it was reported that 74–80% of REM sleep awakenings produced vivid dream recall, compared to only 7–9% of awakenings from NREM sleep[ 56 , 57 ]. It was only natural to conclude that, compared to NREM sleep, the distinct physiology of REM sleep, and especially its fast, low-voltage EEG resembling that of wakefulness, was the reason why we are conscious and dream in REM sleep, and not in NREM sleep[ 29 ]. Indeed, for some time, reports of mental activity upon awakenings from NREM sleep were assumed to be recalls of earlier REM sleep dreams, or considered analogous to sleep talking[ 3 ], or treated as confabulations made up by subjects confused upon awakening[ 9 ] ( Box 1 ). However, when changing the question from “tell me if you had a dream” to “tell me anything that was going through your mind just before you woke up,” reports of conscious experiences in NREM sleep ranged between 23% and 74%[ 9 ]. Subsequent studies demonstrated clearly that NREM sleep awakenings yielded reports of mental activity[ 58 , 59 ].
Specifically, reports from sleep stage N1 are extremely frequent (80–90% of the time), though they are very short[ 60 ]. Usually people report vivid hallucinatory experiences, so-called hypnagogic hallucinations . In contrast to typical dreams, hypnagogic hallucinations are often static - like single snapshots[ 11 , 47 ], and usually do not include a self character[ 14 ]. Some activities performed before sleep (e.g. video games) may influence the content of hypnagogic dreams[ 38 , 61 ]. Awakenings from NREM sleep stages N2 and N3 yield reports about some experienced content 50–70% of the time[ 59 ], although there is great variability throughout the night and between subjects. Early in the night, when stage N3 is prevalent and many large slow waves dominate the EEG, awakenings yield few reports[ 62 ]. Moreover, these reports are often qualitatively different than typical REM sleep reports, being usually short, thought-like, less vivid, less visual and more conceptual, less motorically animated, under greater volitional control, more plausible, more concerned with current issues, less emotional and less pleasant[ 9 , 11 , 63 ]. Also, the average length of REM sleep reports increases with the duration of the REM sleep episode while this is not true for NREM sleep reports[ 62 ]. However, late in the night NREM sleep reports are considerably longer and more hallucinatory. Indeed, 10–30% of all NREM sleep reports are indistinguishable by any criteria from those obtained from REM sleep[ 64 , 65 ]. Since NREM sleep accounts for 75% of total sleep time, this means that full-fledged NREM sleep dreams actually account for a significant portion of all typical dreams.
Thus, the initial equation of a physiological state (REM sleep) with a mental state (dreaming) was incorrect, or at best, an oversimplification. Moreover, neuropsychological evidence indicates that dreaming and REM sleep can be dissociated: forebrain lesions may abolish dreaming and spare REM sleep, whereas brainstem lesions may nearly eliminate overt features of REM sleep without abolishing dreams[ 23 ] ( Box 3 ). But if dream reports can be elicited during any stage of sleep[ 11 , 47 , 59 , 66 , 67 ], and conversely some awakenings may yield no report, no matter in which sleep stage they were obtained[ 59 ], where do we stand today with respect to the relationship between brain activity and consciousness during sleep?
The one thing that seems clear is that we need to move beyond the REM/NREM sleep dichotomy and beyond traditional sleep staging. Though staging is useful, it treats brain activity as uniform in space (only a few electrodes are used) and in time (for 30 sec epochs). Inevitably, subtler features of brain activity, which may well influence the presence, degree, and reportability of consciousness, are missed both in space and in time.
In the spatial domain, increasing evidence suggests that different brain regions may be in different states at the same time. For example, preliminary findings suggest that during sleepwalking, thalamocingulate pathways may be active as in wake, while the rest of the cerebral cortex is in NREM sleep[ 68 ]. A related notion of dissociated states is derived from the study of parasomnias, where wake-like behaviors occur during sleep[ 69 ]. For instance, the study of REM sleep behavior disorder shows that, contrary to common assumptions, wakefulness, REM sleep and NREM sleep may not be mutually exclusive states[ 69 ]. In the current context, it has been suggested that dreaming in NREM sleep is related to ‘covert’ REM processes that occur locally[ 59 ]. Thus, refined spatial analysis using fMRI or high-density EEG (hd-EEG) could potentially identify regionally-specific predictors of dreaming, and possibly indicate, in real time, whether dream reports will be obtained.
In the temporal domain, some attempts have been made to relate transient, phasic activities[ 70 ] to dreaming. For example, various studies have tried to link dream recall to eye movements[ 71 , 72 ], PGO waves[ 73 ], and EEG power bouts in specific frequency bands[ 74 ] but limited success has been achieved, and little has been done for NREM sleep[ 11 , 75 , 76 ]. We now know that slow waves in NREM sleep reflect a slow oscillation of cortical neurons between UP and DOWN states ( Fig. 2 )[ 77 , 78 ]. Perhaps long UP states are necessary for dreaming to occur. This is normally the case in REM sleep since slow waves are absent. As for NREM sleep, we would expect that higher occurrence of recalls, and especially of typical dreams in the morning hours, would reflect longer UP periods upon dissipation of sleep pressure ( Fig. 2 )[ 79 ]. In general, focusing on (rather than avoiding) “gray zones” where it is more difficult to predict whether a dream report will be obtained, for example in early REM sleep or late NREM sleep, may be a promising strategy for identifying psychophysiological correlates that go beyond traditional staging.
Finally, theoretical considerations suggest that the level of consciousness may depend on the brain s ability to integrate information[ 80 ]. Indeed, during wakefulness external perturbations such as TMS pulses (transcranial magnetic stimulation) cause changing patterns of activation across distant interconnected brain regions[ 10 ]. In REM sleep, evoked activity propagates much like it does in wakefulness[ 81 ]. By contrast, in deep SWS early in the night, when consciousness is most likely to fade, the response evoked by TMS remains either local (loss of integration), or spreads nonspecifically (loss of information). Apparently, the brain s capacity for information integration is reduced whenever neurons become bistable between UP and DOWN states. Intriguingly, the brain s response to a TMS pulse may offer a more sensitive measure of the inner state than spontaneous EEG. For example, such perturbations can uncover inherent bistability in short stretches of NREM sleep even when the EEG shows a wake-like low-voltage pattern[ 82 ].
The most obvious difference between dreaming and waking consciousness is the profound disconnection of the dreamer from his current environment. Such disconnection, of course, is a key feature of sleep: by definition a sleeping person shows no meaningful responses to external stimuli, unless they are strong enough to cause an awakening. This feature is known as “high arousal threshold”, and it persists in REM sleep despite its wake-like low-voltage EEG[ 83 ]. Moreover, stimuli not only fail to elicit a behavioral response, but also largely fail to be incorporated in the content of the dream[ 8 , 84 – 86 ] (though some stimuli, such as a spray of water, pressure on the limbs, and meaningful words have a slightly higher chance of incorporation[ 84 , 85 ]). This striking disconnection occurs even when subjects sleep with their eyes taped open and objects are illuminated in front of them[ 8 ]. Surely just before awakening, stimuli such as the sound of an alarm clock can enter our dreams, but when sleep is preserved, such relations are by and large surprisingly weak and dream consciousness is remarkably disconnected from the external environment.
The disconnection of the dreamer poses an intriguing paradox, especially if one considers that dreams involve vivid sensory experiences, and that they can occur upon a state of strong cortical activation. Several possibilities come to mind. For example, it has been suggested that during sleep a thalamic “gate” may close and sensory inputs may not reach the cortex effectively[ 87 ]. However, evoked responses in primary sensory cortices are largely preserved during REM sleep[ 88 , 89 ]. Also, olfactory stimuli are not directly incorporated in dreams[ 90 ], though they are not routed through the thalamus (their emotional valence, however, may affect dreams). A related notion is that of a cortical “gate” leading to diminished inter-cortical propagation[ 91 ], as seems to be the case in the dissociation of primary visual cortex (V1) from high-order visual cortex in REM sleep[ 18 ]. It would be interesting to establish whether direct activation of cortical areas can overcome the disconnection from the environment. For example, can TMS over V1 or area MT bypass thalamic or cortical “gates” and produce sensations of phosphenes or movement in dream consciousness?
An intriguing possibility concerns the putative antagonism between externally oriented cortical networks and internally oriented, default-mode networks[ 92 , 93 ]. Perhaps in dreams intrinsic activity dominates, as it does during stimulus-independent thoughts in wake[ 50 ]. This may occur at the expense of the processing of external stimuli, leading to disconnection from the environment. Indeed, both PET and magnetoencephalography (MEG) suggest that medial prefrontal cortex, a part of the default network, is highly active in REM sleep[ 16 , 17 , 94 ] as it is during wakeful rest ( Fig. 1 ). Conversely, other components of the default network, including posterior cingulate and inferior parietal cortices, are deactivated in REM sleep[ 15 , 16 ], as in highly-engaging waking tasks ( Fig. 1 ). The exact cognitive task associated with the default-mode network is still not well understood[ 95 ] and it may be primarily driven by self-related introspective processes rather than general mind wandering[ 31 , 96 , 97 ]. Indeed, since most nodes of this network are deactivated in REM dreaming and mental imagery[ 98 ], cognitive states that are oriented internally but away from the self do not seem sufficient to elicit activity in this network.
Another possibility is that dreams may be analogous to altered states of consciousness in which attention is profoundly altered, as may be the case in extreme absorption, hypnosis, neglect[ 99 ], and Balint s syndrome, when visual experience may persist for single but unlocalizable objects (simultanagnosia)[ 100 , 101 ]. The reticular thalamic nucleus has been implicated in redirecting attention across modalities[ 102 , 103 ] and its activity in sleep may underlie some aspects of disconnection. It would also be interesting to determine whether neuronal correlates of momentary lapses of attention[ 104 ] occur regularly while dreaming.
Finally, as we have seen, the neuromodulatory milieu changes drastically in sleep ( Fig. 2 ). Specifically, the levels of norepinephrine, serotonin, histamine, and hypocretin are greatly reduced in REM sleep compared to wake, so the presence of one or more of these neuromodulators may be necessary for external stimuli to be incorporated into our stream of consciousness. This search can be narrowed down by considering cataplexy, which affects people with narcolepsy[ 105 ]. Cataplexy is a transient episode of muscle tone loss in which humans report that awareness of external stimuli is preserved, and presumably animals are likewise aware of their environment during cataplectic attacks. Neuromodulatory activity in cataplectic dogs is largely similar to that in REM sleep except that levels of histamine are high, much like during wakefulness[ 105 ]. It thus seems that levels of histamine are correlated with our ability to incorporate sensory stimuli into conscious experience. It would be important to establish whether histamine is indeed necessary for such incorporation, and how it may do so. For instance, could it be that in wakefulness histaminergic tone facilitates transmission of feed-forward sensory inputs in cortical layer 4, at the expense of backward signal propagation?
Whether dreams are generated in a “bottom-up” or a “top-down” manner is a question that has been asked since at least Aristotle[ 106 ]. To put the question in a modern context, do dreams start from activity in low-level sensory areas, which is then interpreted and synthesized by higher-order areas, as is presumably the case in waking perception? Or do they begin as wishes, abstract thoughts, and memories deep in the brain, which are then enriched with perceptual and sensory aspects, as in imagination? Of course, it is possible that such a dichotomy is misguided, and dreams may be best conceptualized as global attractors that emerge simultaneously over many brain areas. However, as we shall see, the available data do indeed suggest that there may be a privileged direction of dream generation.
In the 19th century, sensory experience was often regarded as the source of dreams, which were considered to be an attempt of the mind to interpret somatic nerve-stimuli ( Supplementary Fig. 1 ). A similar notion was later adopted by Henri Beaunis, and recently championed by Allan Hobson ( Table 1 )[ 4 , 11 , 47 ]. According to his AIM model, internally generated signals originating in the brainstem during REM sleep, such as PGO waves, excite visual cortex and are later processed and synthesized by higher-order areas. High levels of acetylcholine in the absence of aminergic neuromodulation may enhance feed-forward transmission and suppress back-propagation[ 3 , 107 ]. By contrast, Freud and some of his followers asserted that dreams originate from psychic motives that are later instantiated as sensory percepts, much like mental imagery[ 5 ].
Deciding between these alternative views will most likely require difficult experiments in which the direction of signal flow during dreaming sleep is evaluated and compared to that during waking perception and imagery[ 108 ] ( Box 4 ). However, various lines of evidence already suggest that dreaming may be more closely related to imagination than to perception. From lesion studies ( Box 3 ) we know that dreaming requires an intact temporo-parieto-occipital junction[ 22 , 23 ] and lesions in this region also affect mental imagery in wakefulness[ 109 ]. Cognitive studies indicate that the skill that maximally correlates with dream recall in adults is visuo-spatial imagery[ 110 ]. In children, dream recall develops hand in hand with visuo-spatial imagery ( Box 2 ). In epileptic patients, direct electrical stimulation in high-order regions such as the medial temporal lobe, rather than in visual cortex, can elicit “dream-like” experiences[ 111 ], although such patients are simultaneously aware of their surroundings. Other evidence comes from lucid dreamers[ 25 ] who report that it is impossible to focus on fine-grain details of visual objects, as is the case in mental imagery[ 112 ]. Perhaps top-down connections lack the anatomical specificity to support detailed representations. The rare occurrences of smells or pain in dreams may also be related to our difficulty in imagining them vividly when awake. However, one important difference between dreaming and mental imagery is that while imagining we are aware that the images are internally generated (preserved reflective thought).
1. Signal propagation in dreams
During wakefulness, sensory responses precede responses in higher-order areas by more than 100ms[ 134 , 135 ]. Does neural activity during dreaming sleep show a similar feed-forward progression as in perception? Or does neural activity propagate backwards, from higher to lower areas, as it is thought to do during imagery? This issue, which is crucial to our understanding of dream generation, could be resolved by examining unit and field potential recordings from the same neuronal populations in wake and REM (or late NREM) sleep in both animals and humans[ 135 ]. One can also apply directional measures of signal propagation (e.g. Granger causality) to hd-EEG data, and check whether the main direction of signal flow inverts between wake and sleep. Finally, one could use TMS with concurrent hd-EEG during both wake and REM sleep, and examine whether there may be a preferential direction of the brain s response to perturbations depending on behavioral state[ 10 ].
2. Functional networks underlying dreaming
So far, most regional studies of brain activity during sleep have employed PET. While PET allows for quantification of cerebral blood flow and comparison across vigilance states, functional MRI (fMRI) offers superior spatial and temporal resolutions. Event-related fMRI has been already used to map brain activity associated with phasic events such as slow waves[ 136 ] and eye movements[ 137 , 138 ]. Studies of functional and effective connectivity[ 139 ] may be especially well suited to map the functional networks underlying dreaming. Notably, perceptual awareness is associated with specific functional connectivity patterns within sensory modalities[ 140 ], between modalities[ 141 ], and with a striking segregation between sensory systems and the default-mode/intrinsic system[ 31 , 93 , 104 ]. Are such connectivity patterns also a hallmark of activity in the dreaming brain? What regional brain activity underlies dreaming in NREM sleep? How do functional networks of mental imagery and dreaming compare in the same subjects? Finally, hd-EEG may be particularly suited for sleep imaging since it (a) allows for relatively undisturbed sleep, (b) upon source modeling can provide a spatial resolution roughly comparable to PET, (c) offers high temporal resolution suitable for evaluating signal propagation, and (d) can be combined with TMS during sleep.
3. Initial steps towards studying dream content
Progress in signal decoding may ultimately enable us to investigate the neural correlates not only of dream form – what is common to all dreams – but also of dream content – what is specific to a particular dream. This can be done, for instance, by using classification techniques applied to fMRI or hd-EEG data[ 142 ]. At least initially, it may be worthwhile to consider some coarse properties of individual dreams, such as the frequency of occurrence of faces or places in a dream report, the amount of movement, or the dominant affective valence. In principle, it should be possible to predict not only the likelihood of a report upon awakening, but also the likelihood of specific features based on preceding brain activity. An important step in this direction would be to identify the contents of internally generated mental imagery using the same approach[ 143 ]. Furthermore, some patients with epilepsy or post-traumatic stress disorder who experience recurring dream contents[ 144 , 145 ] may provide a unique opportunity to relate specific dream content to its neural basis.
If the flow of brain activity during dreaming were shown to be largely backwards, as one would expect in imagery, rather than forwards, as in perception, many of the seemingly bizarre properties of dreams, such as blended characters and scene switches, would be easier to explain, as they are standard features of our imagination. Such a top-down mode may disrupt the encoding of new memories, and thus underlie dream amnesia. In addition, top-down mental imagery could obstruct the processing of incoming stimuli and disconnect us from the environment. If this view is correct, waking consciousness is more like watching the news in real time, while dreaming is more like watching a movie created by an imaginative director[ 81 ]. As in some B-movies, the director is not particularly choosey and any actor, dress, means of transportation, or object that is readily available will do. Albert Einstein said that “imagination points to all we might yet discover and create”, and indeed, dreaming may turn out to be the purest form of our imagination.
In summary, dream consciousness is remarkably similar to waking consciousness, though there are several intriguing differences in volition, self-awareness and reflection, affect, and memory, and there is great variability between individual dreams. The neurophysiology of REM sleep, and in particular recent insights into its regional activity patterns, offers a useful starting point for relating dream phenomenology to underlying brain activity. However, the initial equation of REM sleep with dreaming has been shown to be inaccurate. Thus, it is time we moved beyond sleep stages when trying to link dream consciousness to neuronal events, and focused on more subtle features of brain activity in space and time. Our profound disconnection from the external environment when dreaming poses a central unsolved paradox, the answer to which may be instrumental for understanding dreams. Converging evidence from multiple fields of study, including phenomenology, development, neuropsychology, functional imaging, and neurophysiology, support the notion that dreaming may be closely related to imagination, where brain activity presumably flows in a “top-down” manner. Viewing dreams as a powerful form of imagination can help explain many of their unique features, such as sudden transitions, uncertainty about people and places, poor subsequent recall, disconnection from the environment, and offers testable predictions for future studies.
Acknowledgments.
We apologize to those whose work was not cited because of space constraints. We thank Michal Harel, Lior Fisch, and Vlad Vyazovskiy for help with figures; Chiara Cirelli, Rafi Malach, Simone Sarasso, Brady Riedner, and Fabio Ferrarelli for helpful discussions and comments; our anonymous reviewers for valuable suggestions. Y.N. is supported by an EMBO long term fellowship and the Brainpower for Israel Fund. G.T is supported by an NIH Director’s Award DP1 OD000579 and NIH Conte Center Award P20 MH077967.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
I'm going to organize in August 207 independent expedition to Kuril Islands (Kunashir and Iturup) with access to the islands and escape with a m/v from Korsakov (Yuzhno-Sakhalinsk).
Is it possible?
I know several persons that have traveled from Sakhalin to the Kurils . Note that :
- You might need a Border Pass
- Logistics to travel to Kuril are nor simple. You can stay longer than expected over there due to logistics.
- You might check several of the travel agencies in Yuzho that offer support for this expeditions at : http://www.sakh.com
This topic has been closed to new posts due to inactivity.
Overview | Map | Directions | Satellite | Photo Map |
Overview | Map | Directions |
Satellite | Photo Map |
Tap on the map to travel |
Sakhalin regional museum.
Explore these curated destinations.
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Neuroscience research has the potential to support and refine models of motivation and cognitive skill. It may play a pivotal role in developing classroom interventions and understanding non-cognitive skills (e.g., mindset). ... Most empirical research on growth mindset and intrinsic motivation has focused on behavioral methods and self-reports ...
Research data. Cognitive neuroscience seeks generalizable theories explaining the relationship between behavioral, physiological and mental states. In pursuit of such theories, we propose a ...
The theory of multiple intelligences (MI) has motivated the present investigation. Gardner [3], [4] redefined intelligence as the ability to solve problems or create products of value in a culture or community. Using this broad, common sense definition and eight criteria* that cover a range of empirical evidence (e.g., neuroscience, psychometric and evolutionary evidence, and atypical ...
Intrinsic motivation would seem to be an especially ripe topic for neuroscience precisely because of the large body of empirical data that has already been garnered at the experiential and behavioral levels of analysis. Our purpose of this review article is to survey the progress of neuroscience research on intrinsic motivation. Because ...
Read the latest Research articles from Nature Neuroscience. Skip to main content. ... Nature Neuroscience (Nat Neurosci) ISSN 1546-1726 (online) ISSN 1097-6256 (print) nature.com sitemap ...
Educational neuroscience is an interdisciplinary field exploring the effects of education on the human brain and promotes the translation of research findings to brain-based pedagogies and policies . The brain is the target organ of education. Education is thought to influence brain development [2,3] and health, even as the brain ages [4,5 ...
Cognitive neuroscience research on conceptual knowledge often is discussed with respect to "embodiment" or "grounding." We tried to disentangle at least three distinct claims made using these terms. One of these, the view that concepts are entirely reducible to sensory-motor representations, is untenable and diminishing in the literature.
The primary mission of Behavioral Neuroscience® is to publish original research articles as well as reviews in the broad field of the neural bases of behavior. We seek empirical papers reporting novel results that provide insight into the mechanisms by which nervous systems produce and are affected by behavior.
Top 100 in Neuroscience. This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2020. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers showcase valuable research ...
Clark makes a convincing case for the merits of conceptualizing brains as hierarchical prediction machines. This perspective has the potential to provide an elegant and powerful general theory of brain function, but it will ultimately stand or fall with evidence from basic neuroscience research. Her …
Investigating the relationship between brain structure and function is a central endeavor for neuroscience research. Yet, the mechanisms shaping this relationship largely remain to be elucidated and are highly debated. ... Empirical and simulated windowed FC were computed on individual subjects using sliding time-windows (increment of 20 s) of ...
A major thrust of cognitive neuroscience is the elucidation of structure-function relationships in the human brain. Over the last several years, functional neuroimaging has risen in prominence relative to the lesion studies that formed the historical core of work in this field. ... an empirical study of impact in cognitive neuroscience J Cogn ...
Neurophenomenology presents a methodological strategy that takes a vantage point from phenomenology, that is the conscious experience of the first-person-perspective, to especially research consciousness and its connection to the neuronal level of the third-person-perspective of the observational-experimental empirical research of neuroscience ...
Consciousness is now a well-established field of empirical research. A large body of experimental results has been accumulated and is steadily growing. In parallel, many Theories of Consciousness (ToCs) have been proposed. These theories are diverse in nature, ranging from computational to neurophysiological and quantum theoretical approaches.
Author summary Biomedical science, psychology, and many other fields may be suffering from a serious replication crisis. In order to gain insight into some factors behind this crisis, we have analyzed statistical information extracted from thousands of cognitive neuroscience and psychology research papers. We established that the statistical power to discover existing relationships has not ...
Neuroscience News posts science research news from labs, universities, hospitals and news departments around the world. Science articles cover neuroscience, psychology, AI, robotics, neurology, brain cancer, mental health, machine learning, autism, Parkinson's, Alzheimer's, brain research, depression and other sciences.
Recent years have seen a blossoming of theories about the biological and physical basis of consciousness. Good theories guide empirical research, allowing us to interpret data, develop new ...
Several dozen Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience at the University of Rochester faculty, researchers, and trainees from programs including Neuroscience, Brain and Cognitive Science, and Toxicology traveled to Chicago for the annual Society for Neuroscience (SfN) Conference. According to SfN, more than 30,000 people regularly attend the annual conference.
Abstract. This paper explores memory from a cognitive neuroscience perspective and examines associated neural mechanisms. It examines the different types of memory: working, declarative, and non-declarative, and the brain regions involved in each type. The paper highlights the role of different brain regions, such as the prefrontal cortex in ...
Join Us in San Diego for Neuroscience 2025! Meeting Dates: Saturday, November 15-Wednesday, November 19. Location: San Diego Convention Center. Each year, scientists from around the world congregate to discover new ideas, share their research, and experience the best the field has to offer. Attend so you can: present research, network with ...
The Clinical Neuroscience MSc is a pioneering Master's programme taught at the UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology. UCL is the top-ranked university in the UK for research power in Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience according to the UK's Research Excellence Framework 2021.
The Dementia: Causes, Treatments and Research (Neuroscience) MSc, offered by the UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, tackles one of the biggest global health problems facing society today. It provides research-oriented and cutting-edge training in the study of dementia and its scientific basis, delivered by international leaders in the science and practice of dementia.
Empirical research in the US often focuses on IC deficiencies, suggesting a need for empirical indicators of robust IC (Chen et al., Citation 2016). ICQ measurement methods include disclosure indices and content analysis, which have limitations such as self-construction and potential bias (Hassan & Marston, Citation 2010 ; Chalmers et al ...
Sakhalin (Russian: Сахали́н, IPA: [səxɐˈlʲin]) is an island in Northeast Asia.Its north coast lies 6.5 km (4.0 mi) off the southeastern coast of Khabarovsk Krai in Russia, while its southern tip lies 40 kilometres (25 mi) north of Japan's Hokkaido.An island of the West Pacific, Sakhalin divides the Sea of Okhotsk to its east from the Sea of Japan to its southwest.
Yuzhno-Sakhalinsk began as a small Russian settlement called Vladimirovka, founded by convicts in 1882. [2] The Treaty of Portsmouth in 1905, which brought an end to the Russo-Japanese War of 1904-1905, awarded the southern half of the Sakhalin Island to Japan.Vladimirovka was renamed Toyohara (meaning "bountiful plain"), and was the prefect capital of the Japanese Karafuto Prefecture.
Contemporary dream research. Although dreams have fascinated us since the dawn of time, their rigorous, scientific study is a recent development[1-4] (Supplementary Fig. 1).In The interpretation of dreams [] Freud predicted that "Deeper research will one day trace the path further and discover an organic basis for the mental event."Recent work, which we review in this article, begins to ...
Answer 1 of 2: I'm going to organize in August 207 independent expedition to Kuril Islands (Kunashir and Iturup) with access to the islands and escape with a m/v from Korsakov (Yuzhno-Sakhalinsk). Is it possible?
Yuzhno-Sakhalinsk, also spelled Uzno-Sakhalinsk and previously known in Japanese as Toyohara, is the largest city and capital of Sakhalin Oblast, in the Russian Far East, with a population of around 173,000. Photo: Alkhimov Maxim, CC BY-SA 4.0. Photo: Wikimedia, CC BY-SA 2.0. Photo: Maarten, CC BY 2.0. Ukraine is facing shortages in its brave ...