An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

PLOS ONE logo

The association between role model presence and self-regulation in early adolescence: A cross-sectional study

Miharu Nakanishi

Syudo yamasaki, shuntaro ando, yuko morimoto, shinya fujikawa, yusuke takahashi, toshi a furukawa, marcus richards, mariko hiraiwa-hasegawa, kiyoto kasai, atsushi nishida.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

* E-mail: [email protected]

Contributed equally.

Received 2019 Jun 12; Accepted 2019 Sep 7; Collection date 2019.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Self-regulation is the capacity to regulate attention, emotion, and behaviour to pursue long-term goals. The current study examined the associations between role model presence and self-regulation during early adolescence, controlling for hopefulness, using a large population-based data set from the Tokyo Teen Cohort study.

Adolescents, aged 12 years, identified a role model using a single item on a paper questionnaire: ‘Who is the person you most look up to?’ Level of hopefulness was also assessed using a single question: ‘To what extent do you feel hopeful about the future of your life?’ Trained investigators evaluated self-regulation.

Of 2550 adolescents, 2279 (89.4%) identified a role model. After adjusting for level of hopefulness, identifying a role model was associated with higher levels of self-regulation in comparison to indications of no role model. Hopeful future expectations were also associated with higher self-regulation; however, the beta coefficient was smaller than role model presence in the multivariate linear regression analysis.

Conclusions

Role model presence was significantly associated with higher self-regulation among early adolescents. Educational environments should focus on support for adolescents with no role models.

Introduction

Adolescence is a phase of human growth and development that takes place between childhood and adulthood [ 1 ]. Psychological maladjustments during adolescence, such as low self-esteem [ 2 ], depression [ 3 ], and suicidal behaviour [ 4 ], have an adverse influence on health and well-being in adulthood. Recent developments in research have suggested a need for an increased emphasis on strength and resilience-based approaches to adolescent health and positive youth development [ 5 , 6 ].

Self-regulation is a significant adjustment indicator of positive youth development [ 7 , 8 ]. Self-regulation is defined as the capacity to override natural and automatic tendencies, desires, or behaviours to pursue long-term goals, even at the expense of short-term desires [ 9 ]. Higher levels of self-regulation earlier in life predict later academic achievement [ 10 ], improved health, and psychological well-being [ 11 ]. Aside from positive psychological development, self-regulation is also related to internalising (anxiety, depression, and somatic complaints) and externalising (aggression, conduct problems, and delinquency) psychopathologies [ 12 ]. However, self-regulation is positive when an individual engages in goal-directed behaviour [ 13 ]. High levels of self-regulation have been observed in female adolescents [ 10 , 14 ] and are associated with early language skills [ 14 ], high maternal education levels [ 14 ], and hopeful expectations for the future [ 15 ]. However, the correlates and sources of self-regulation have been less frequently explored.

Role model presence is a form of positive psychological functioning, which can play a central role in shaping self-regulation during early adolescence. Role models are individuals whom adolescents admire. Since identity formation is a central focus during adolescence, individuals often look to adults to determine what is appropriate and acceptable behaviour, as well as to identify role models whom they would like to resemble [ 16 ]. Role model presence has been related to reduced health risk behaviours in adolescents [ 17 – 20 ]. In addition to role model presence, type of role model is associated with health-related behaviours [ 21 ]. Identifying an individual known to the adolescent as a role model is particularly associated with higher self-esteem [ 17 ].

Hopeful future expectations can facilitate positive development for adolescents [ 22 ]. Hope represents an individual’s ability to formulate or envision goals that motivate the individual to plan a course of behaviour, and the ability to see a plan or process by which the goals might be accomplished [ 23 ]. Hopeful future expectations have been suggested to play an important role in the development of self-regulation across adolescence [ 15 ]. As hopefulness stems from a sense of purpose in life [ 24 ], role model presence may precede hopeful future expectations.

The associations between role model presence and self-regulation after adjusting for level of hopefulness have not been previously examined in early adolescence. Clarifying the association between role model presence and self-regulation will provide a better understanding of the processes involved in positive youth development.

The present study aimed to investigate the associations between role model presence and self-regulation, controlling for hopefulness, among early adolescents, using a large population-based cohort, the Tokyo Teen Cohort (TTC). We hypothesised that self-regulation would be higher among adolescents who could identify a role model, even after controlling for their level of hopeful future expectations.

Materials and methods

The present study used a cross-sectional design, based on data from the TTC. The TTC is a population-based longitudinal cohort study investigating the health and development of adolescents in three metropolitan areas of Tokyo, Japan [ 25 ]. The TTC consists of a baseline survey of 10-year-old children, a two-year follow-up survey and supplementary information provided by respondents’ families. The present study used data from the first follow-up survey, when the respondents were 12 years old. Further details of the TTC are available in the literature [ 26 , 27 ].

During the first follow-up survey, adolescents and their primary caregivers were asked to complete a self-administered, anonymous questionnaire. Trained investigators visited each participant’s home twice between October 2012 and January 2015. After obtaining informed consent from both the adolescents and their caregivers during the first visit, participants were asked to complete the paper-based questionnaire. During the second visit, assessments were conducted using questionnaires and interviews. Questionnaires were completed anonymously and returned to the research team in closed envelopes.

All procedures involving human participants were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the associated institutional research committees and in adherence to the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments, or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all adolescent respondents and their caregivers included in the study. All protocols were approved by the research ethics committees of the University of Tokyo Faculty of Medicine (approval no. 3150, 10057, and 10069), Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science (approval no. 12–35), and the Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI) (approval no. 2012002).

Participants

The baseline TTC survey included 3171 adolescents who were born between September 2002 and August 2004. Of the 3171 adolescents, 3007 (94.8%) participated in the first follow-up survey two years later.

Measurements

The outcome variable in this study was self-regulation. Levels of self-regulation were measured by investigators. An investigator was asked to evaluate the self-control behaviours of each adolescent using a three-category response scale comparing their behaviours against those of a ‘normal child’ at age 12. Investigators completed a training course on self-regulation, including trial evaluations of at least 50 children. The self-regulation measure consisted of four items on behaviours related to attitude toward work, concentration, neatness in work, and daydreaming at home [ 28 , 29 ]. The Japanese version of the instrument was developed as part of the current study. Total score was calculated using the sum of responses to the four items, with a range of 0 to 8. Higher scores indicated higher levels of self-regulation. In the present study, the internal consistency of the self-regulation scale was moderate, with a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.76.

The primary explanatory variable in this study was role model presence. Role model presence/absence was assessed by asking the participants a single open-ended question: ‘Who is the person you most look up to?’ Persons identified as role models were categorised as either individuals known to the adolescent (familial and non-familial), or figures primarily known through the media. Sub-categories referring to type of media figure were developed by a research panel, which included a registered nurse and clinical psychologist [ 21 ], based on the occupation groups for career aspirations identified in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children [ 30 ]. Classification of each identified role model was performed by the research team (KE, in consultation with MN and SY). The results were compared, and differences were discussed until an agreement was reached regarding the classification of the identified role model.

The primary covariate in this study was hopeful future expectations. Level of hopefulness was assessed using a single question: ‘To what extent do you feel hopeful about the future of your life?’ Participants were asked to respond on a visual analogue scale from 0 ( not at all ) to 10 ( extremely ).

Statistical analysis

The frequency of identification of each type of role model was calculated. Based on their responses, the adolescents were divided into four groups: (1) those identifying a familial individual; (2) those identifying a non-familial individual; (3) those identifying a media figure role model; and (4) those identifying no role model.

Level of hopefulness, according to role model presence and type of role model identified, were compared using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). A Bonferonni correction was used for multiple pairwise tests.

Levels of self-regulation according to role model presence and type of role model identified were also compared using a one-way ANOVA. Since gender differences in self-regulation have been reported [ 14 , 15 ], the total score differences for self-regulation between males and females were compared using a Student’s t test. A pairwise correlation coefficient was calculated between hopeful future expectations and self-regulation.

Finally, a multiple linear regression analysis was conducted, using self-regulation as a dependent variable, and role model presence and type of role model as independent variables. Adolescent age and gender were included in the model as covariates. Hopeful future expectations score was then added to the multivariate model. In the regression analyses, full information maximum likelihood was used for estimating missing data [ 31 ].

All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata 15.1 (StataCorp, Texas) for Windows. The two-tailed significance level was set at .05.

Of the 3007 participants, 2550 with complete information regarding self-regulation were included in analyses. The mean age of the 2550 adolescents included in the analysis ( M = 12.1 years) was significantly lower than that of the 457 excluded from the analysis [ M = 13.3; t (610.2) = 9.50, p < .001]. Of the 2550 adolescent participants included in analyses, 1352 (53.0%) were male. The proportion of males was not significantly different from those excluded from the analysis [χ 2 (1) = 0.04, p = .843].

Of the 2550 adolescents, 2279 identified a role model. The types of role models are shown in Table 1 . In the 2279 responses, participants identified role models who were family members ( N = 1114, 43.7%), non-familial known individuals ( N = 404, 15.8%), and individuals known primarily through media ( N = 761, 29.8%). Of the 2550 adolescents, 154 (6.0%) answered ‘no one’ when asked to identify a role model. No response to the question was given by 117 (4.6%) of the participants. Significantly more male adolescents identified a figure known through the media when compared to females [ χ 2 (1) = 87.84, p < .001].

Table 1. Role model presence and type ( N = 2550).

Of those who reported family members as role models ( N = 1114), 976 (87.6%) identified their parents. Non-familial known individuals, indicated as role models by 404 of the adolescents, included friends ( N = 184, 45.5%) and teachers ( N = 183, 45.3%).

A qualitative review of responses identifying figures known via media as role models ( N = 761) yielded 14 sub-categories based on occupational groups. Types of figure role models are presented in Table 2 . Males were more likely to identify a sports player as a role model ( N = 208, 40.9%), whereas females were more likely to identify a historical figure who was devoted to humanity ( N = 68, 26.9%) or an artist ( N = 51, 20.2%).

Table 2. Types of figure role models among adolescents (N = 761).

Of the 2550 adolescents, 2539 responded to the question regarding hopefulness ( M = 7.3, SD = 2.1). Level of hopefulness of future expectations according to role model is summarised in Table 3 . Hopefulness was significantly higher in adolescents who identified a role model than those who identified no role model.

Table 3. Level of hopefulness of future expectations according to role model presence and type.

SD: standard deviation.

Hopefulness of future expectations was assessed using a visual analogue scale from 0 to 10.

a,b,c,d,e significant difference ( p < .05) between the same alphabet, with Bonferroni correction.

The 2550 adolescents were rated by investigators with a mean self-regulation score of 5.7 (SD = 1.2). Self-regulation was significantly lower in males than females [ t (2505.1) = 2.55, p = .011]. A significant positive correlation between hopeful future expectations and self-regulation ( r = 0.07, p = .001) was observed.

Level of self-regulation according to role model presence and type is presented in Table 4 . Self-regulation was significantly higher in adolescents who identified a role model than those who identified no role model.

Table 4. Level of self-regulation according to role model presence and type.

Self-regulation was assessed using the 4-item scale of self-control behaviours.

a,b,c significant difference ( p < .05) between the same alphabet, Bonferroni correction.

Results of a multivariate linear regression analysis are shown in Table 5 . After adjusting for age and sex, the analysis showed that adolescents with familial, non-familial, or media figure role models demonstrated higher total scores for self-regulation than those with no role models. Additionally, female participants demonstrated higher levels of self-regulation when compared to male adolescents. When adding hopeful future expectations as an independent variable in the model, the total score rating of self-regulation was higher in adolescents with a higher level of hopefulness. The significant positive association between role model and self-regulation remained after accounting for hopeful future expectations.

Table 5. Estimated coefficients (Beta) predicting level of self-regulation.

N = 2550; full information maximum likelihood estimation was used.

CI: confidence interval.

* Significant at p < .05.

The present study is the first to indicate positive associations between role model presence and self-regulation in early adolescence. Specifically, in cases where adolescents identified a role model, higher levels of self-regulation were observed. Hopeful future expectations were also associated with higher self-regulation; however, the association between role model presence and self-regulation remained significant even after adjusting for level of hopeful future expectations.

Family members were the most commonly reported role model. However, male adolescents were more likely than females to identify media figures as role models, rather than known individuals. This pattern of role model identification is consistent with that observed in the United States [ 18 , 21 ]. The male adolescents’ responses may have reflected representations of gender in media texts [ 32 ], since young people tend to identify more strongly with gender-matched role models [ 21 , 33 ]. Since some media figure role model types have been associated with increased health risk behaviours [ 21 ], further examination of the potentially negative effects of role models on health-related behaviours and self-regulation in adolescents is required.

Adolescents who have identified a role model have higher levels of self-regulation, as well as higher levels of hopeful future expectations. Both role model presence and hopeful future expectations were positively related to self-regulation in the final regression model. Therefore, role models may provide guidance in the formation of future expectations, contributing to higher self-regulation in adolescents in turn. The results of our study indicate that both media figure role models and known role models, whether familial or non-familial individuals, are positively associated with self-regulation. This finding emphasises the importance of the presence of role models external to adolescents’ living environments, in addition to known individuals. In future, to enhance self-regulation, educational environments should focus on providing support for adolescents with no role models. In this study, some of the adolescents who did not respond to the role model question may have reacted differently if they were asked to identify a role model in an interview setting. In some cases, lack of response to the open question could have reflected a hesitation to identify ‘no one’ as a role model or being unable to think of a role model. This implies that there is a need for psychological support among these adolescents.

The greater self-regulation observed among female participants is consistent with the findings of previous, similar studies [ 10 , 14 ]. Greater self-regulation in females may arise from cultural stereotypes, based on which females are more likely to mentally and behaviourally meet the needs of others [ 34 ]. Other factors relating to self-regulation among female adolescents have also been suggested, including the timing of rapid gains in the ability to regulate behaviour, early language skills, maternal education levels [ 14 ], and positive expectations for the future [ 15 ]. Our study contributes to the literature by examining the intrinsic factors related to self-regulation at an earlier developmental stage of adolescence.

Some limitations of the present study should be considered. This study utilised cross-sectional survey data which did not allow for the assessment of the stability of role model presence and type of role model. This may be problematic, as adolescents’ future expectations can change with age [ 35 ]. In the future, longitudinal changes in role model identification, future expectations, and self-regulation should be investigated as part of the developmental process. Although the Japanese version of the instrument for measuring self-regulation was developed based on well-established measures, further examination may be warranted to establish its validity.

Despite the limitations, the present study is the first to indicate that a positive association exists between role model presence and self-regulation among early adolescents. Our results suggest that identifying a role model be beneficial to the development of high self-regulation among early adolescents. Furthermore, there is a stronger association between role model presence and self-regulation than between hopeful future expectations and self-regulation. In the future, educational environments should focus on providing support for adolescents with no role models. Longitudinal analyses will allow for further investigation of change in role model identification, future expectations, and self-regulation across the course of development.

Supporting information

Acknowledgments.

We are thankful to all the adolescents who participated in this study.

Data Availability

Data cannot be shared publicly because of ethical restrictions imposed by the institutional ethics committee, as data contain sensitive information on young adolescents. However, the data can be made available from the Tokyo Teen Cohort Data Operating Committee for all interested researchers upon requests sent to the committee. The initial contact for request should be addressed to Translational Research Planning and Management Office [ [email protected] ].

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology of Japan [grant numbers JP23118002, JP16H06398, JP16H06395]. This work was partly supported by UTokyo Center for Integrative Science of Human Behavior (CiSHuB) and the International Research Center for Neurointelligence (WPI-IRCN) at The University of Tokyo Institutes for Advanced Study (UTIAS). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

  • 1. Sawyer SM, Azzopardi PS, Wickremarathne MDS, Patto GC. The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2018;2(3):223–228. 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 2. Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB, Moffitt TE, Robins RW, Poulton R, Caspi A. Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Dev Psychol. 2006;42(2):381–390. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM, Beautrais AL. Suicidal behaviour in adolescence and subsequent mental health outcomes in young adulthood. Psychol Med. 2005;35(7):983–993. 10.1017/s0033291704004167 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM, Beautrais AL. Subthreshold depression in adolescence and mental health outcomes in adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(1):66–72. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.1.66 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Larson RW. Toward a psychology of positive youth development. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):170–183. 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.170 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Lerner RM, Almerigi JB, Theokas C, Lerner JV. Positive youth development: A view of the issues. J Early Adolesc. 2005;25(1):10–16. 10.1177/0272431604273211 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 7. Gestsdottir S, Bowers E, von Eye A, Napolitano CM, Lerner RM. Intentional self-regulation in middle adolescence: the emerging role of loss-based selection in positive youth development. J Youth Adolesc. 2010:39(7):764–782. 10.1007/s10964-010-9537-2 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Hadjicharalambous MZ, Fanti KA. Self regulation, cognitive capacity and risk taking: Investigating heterogeneity among adolescents with callous-unemotional traits. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2018;49(3):331–340. 10.1007/s10578-017-0753-9 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Duckworth AL, Grant H, Loew B, Oettingen G, Gollwitzer PM. Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions. Educ Psychol. 2011;31(1):17–26. 10.1080/01443410.2010.506003 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Matthews JS, Ponitz CC, Morrison FJ. Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. J Educ Psychol. 2009;101(3):689–704. 10.1037/a0014240 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11. Howard SJ, Williams KE. Early self-regulation, early self-regulatory change, and their longitudinal relations to adolescents’ academic, health, and mental well-being outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39(6):489–496. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000578 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Laceulle OM, Veenstra R, Vollebergh WAM, Ormel J. Sequences of maladaptation: Preadolescent self-regulation, adolescent negative social interactions, and young adult psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2019;31(1):279–292. 10.1017/S0954579417001808 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Duckworth AL, Gross JJ. Self-control and grit: Related but separable determinants of success. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(5):319–325. 10.1177/0963721414541462 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. Montroy JJ, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE, McClelland MM, Morrison FJ. The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(11):1744–1762. 10.1037/dev0000159 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Schmid KL, Phelps E, Lerner RM. Constructing positive futures: Modeling the relationship between adolescents’ hopeful future expectations and intentional self-regulation in predicting positive youth development. J Adolesc. 2011;34(6):1127–1135. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.07.009 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Erikson EH. Identity, youth and crisis New York: W. W. Norton Company; 1968. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Yancey AK, Siegel JM, McDaniel KL. Role models, ethic identify, and health-risk behaviors in urban adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(1):55–61. 10.1001/archpedi.156.1.55 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Mistry R, McCarthy WJ, Yancey AK, Lu Y, Patel M. Resilience and patterns of health risk behaviors in California adolescents. Prev Med. 2009;48(3):291–297. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.12.013 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Hurd NM, Zimmerman MA, Xue Y. Negative adult influences and the protective effects of role models: A study with urban adolescents. J Youth and Adolesc. 2009;38(6):777–789. 10.1007/s10964-008-9296-5 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. Kim HJ, Min JY, Min KB, Lee TJ, Yoo S. Relationship among family environment, self-control, friendship quality, and adolescents' smartphone addiction in South Korea: Findings from nationwide data. PLoS One. 2018;13(2): e0190896 10.1371/journal.pone.0190896 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Yancy AK, Grant D, Kurosky S, Kravitz-Wirtz N, Mistry R. Role modeling, risk, and resilience in California adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2011;48(1):36–43. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.05.001 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. Callina KS, Johnson SK, Buckingham MH, Lerner RM. Hope in context: developmental profiles of trust, hopeful future expectations, and civic engagement across adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2014;43(6):869–883. 10.1007/s10964-014-0096-9 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23. Snyder CR, Harris C, Anderson JR, Holleran SA, Irving LM, Sigmon ST. The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60(4):570–585. 10.1037//0022-3514.60.4.570 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Stoddard SA, Pierce J. Promoting positive future expectations during adolescence: the role of assets. Am J Community Psychol. 2015;56(3–4):332–341. 10.1007/s10464-015-9754-7 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Ando S, Nishida A, Yamasaki S, Koike S, Morimoto Y, Hoshino A, et al. Cohort profile: The Tokyo Teen Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2019, 10.1093/ije/dyz033 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Kanata S, Koike S, Ando S, Nishida A, Usami S, Yamasaki S, et al. Enuresis and hyperactivity-inattention in early adolescence: Findings from a population-based survey in Tokyo (Tokyo Early Adolescence Survey). PLoS One. 2016;11(7):e0158786 10.1371/journal.pone.0158786 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 27. Yamasaki S, Ando S, Koike S, Usami S, Endo K, French P, et al. Dissociation mediates the relationship between peer victimization and hallucinatory experiences among early adolescents. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2016;4:18–23. 10.1016/j.scog.2016.04.001 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Xu MK, Jones PB, Barnett JH, Gaysina D, Kuh D, Croudace TJ, Richards M. Adolescent self-organization predicts midlife memory in a prospective birth cohort study. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(4):958–968. 10.1037/a0033787 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Nishida A, Xu KM, Croudace T, Jones PB, Barnett J, Richards M. Adolescent self-control predicts midlife hallucinatory experiences: 40-year follow-up of a national birth cohort. Schizophr Bull 2014;40(6):1543–1551. 10.1093/schbul/sbu050 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Australian Institute of Family Studies. The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children Annual Statistical Report 2016. Melbourne: AIFS; 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Cham H, Reshetnyak E, Rosenfeld B, Breitbart W. Full information maximum likelihood estimation for latent variable interactions with incomplete indicators. Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(1):12–30. 10.1080/00273171.2016.1245600 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Steinke J. Cultural representations of gender and science: Portrayals of female scientists and engineers in popular films. Sci Commun. 2005;27(1):27–63. 10.1177/1075547005278610 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Hoffner C, Buchanan M. Young adults' wishful identification with television characters: The role of perceived similarity and character attributes. Media Psychol. 2005;7(4):325–351. 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0704_2 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Stoker JI, Van der Velde M, Lammers J. Factors relating to managerial stereotypes: The role of gender of the employee and the manager and management gender ratio. J Bus Psychol. 2012;27(1):31–42. 10.1007/s10869-011-9210-0 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 35. Beal SJ, Crockett LJ, Peugh J. Adolescents’ changing future expectations predict the timing of adult role transitions. Dev Psychol 2016;52(10):1606–1618. 10.1037/dev0000189 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Decision Letter 0

Geilson lima santana.

PONE-D-19-16626

Role model presence as a shaper of early adolescents’ self-regulation: A population-based study

Dear Dr. Nakanishi,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Sep 19 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Geilson Lima Santana, M.D., Ph.D.

Academic Editor

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include additional information regarding the survey or questionnaire used in the study and ensure that you have provided sufficient details that others could replicate the analyses. For instance, if you developed a questionnaire as part of this study and it is not under a copyright more restrictive than CC-BY, please include a copy, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information.

3. Thank you for including your ethics statement: "All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committees (Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, the University of Tokyo, and SOKENDAI) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from both all adolescents and their caregivers included in the study."

a) Please amend your current ethics statement to confirm that your named institutional review board or ethics committee specifically approved this study.

b) Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”).

For additional information about PLOS ONE ethical requirements for human subjects research, please refer to http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research .

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is an interesting study that examined an association between role model presence and self-regulation using a large sample. Self-regulation is an important in human development process. Clarifying the relationship between role model presence and self-regulation appears to contribute on future education and the relevant intervention. I found the only two revision points and other two discretionary revision points. I listed them below.

1. Title, hypothesis, and discussion

The authors can clarify the primary aim of this study and how hopefulness was treated in the study. The title indicates that the primary aim was to test association between role model presence and self-regulation, although the two hypotheses mentioned hopefulness. Also, the discussion focused mostly on role model and self-regulation, rather than hopefulness. If the hopefulness was important for this study, the authors can add their assumption of the significant results for hopefulness in the section of Discussion.

2. Limitation

It is suggested that the authors mention the validity of scales/measures which assessed main variables in this study.

Discretionary revision

While the study was part of well-deigned large-scale cohort study in Japan, the present study employed a cross-sectional design. This indicates that the study could not create evidence on direction between exposure and outcome. Therefore, the terms “shaper” would not be appropriate. This term problem may apply to the same or similar terms throughout the manuscript. In addition, STROBE guideline (e.g. von Elm et al, BMJ, 2007) recommends the authors to write the study design in the title. The authors can reconsider the title. An example below;

An association between role model presence and self-regulation in early adolescents: a cross-sectional study.

2. Statistical analysis

In the final analysis, it appears that the authors included the variable for hopefulness, like a control variable or a co-variate. If the authors have two clear hypotheses about testing two associations between role model presence and self-regulation and between role model presence and hopefulness, each hypothetical relationship can be separately tested in the different regression models, because each relationship may theoretically have the different potential confounders for which the authors should be adjusted. On the other hand, if the authors want to examine the association between three variables including role model presence, self-regulation and hopefulness, together, structural equation modelling (SEM) would be appropriate as the final modelling analysis.

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article ( what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy .

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/ . PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at [email protected] . Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Author response to Decision Letter 0

Collection date 2019.

29 Aug 2019

Comments from Reviewer 1

General comment

This is an interesting study that examined an association between role model presence and self-regulation using a large sample. Self-regulation is an important in human development process. Clarifying the relationship between role model presence and self-regulation appears to contribute on future education and the relevant intervention. I found the only two revision points and other two discretionary revision points. I listed them below.

Response the general comment

Thank you for your comments. We have incorporated the suggested changes into the manuscript to the best of my ability.

Response to the comment 1

Thank you for your comment. As suggested by the comment 1 and 4, our primary aim was to test association between role model presence and self-regulation. Therefore we have modified abstract, Introduction, Materials and Methods, and Discussion to make the point clear.

Abstract, page 4 line 49-52: The current study examined the associations between role model presence and self-regulation during early adolescence, controlling for hopefulness, using a large population-based data set from the Tokyo Teen Cohort study.

Introduction, page 7 line 101-107: Hopeful future expectations have been suggested to play an important role in the development of self-regulation across adolescence [15]. As hopefulness stems from a sense of purpose in life [24], role model presence may precede hopeful future expectations. The associations between role model presence and self-regulation after adjusting for level of hopefulness have not been previously examined in early adolescence.

Introduction, page 8 line 109-116: The present study aimed to investigate the associations between role model presence and self-regulation, controlling for hopefulness, among adolescents, using a large population-based cohort, the Tokyo Teen Cohort (TTC). We hypothesised that self-regulation would be higher among adolescents who could identify a role model, even after controlling for their level of hopeful future expectations.

Materials and Methods, page 11 line 173-174: The primary covariate in this study was hopeful future expectations.

Discussion, page 19 line 262-268: The present study is the first to indicate positive associations between role model presence and self-regulation in early adolescence. Specifically, in case where adolescents identified a role model, higher levels of self-regulation were observed. Hopeful future expectations were also associated with higher self-regulation; however, the association between role model presence and self-regulation remained significant even after adjusting for level of hopeful future expectations.

Response to the comment 2

Thank you for the suggestion. As suggested by the editor’s comment 2, we have explained that the Japanese version of instrument for self-regulation was developed as part of our study in page 10 line 157-158. Thus, we have added the mention regarding validity to limitations.

Page 21 line 306 - page 22 line 308: Although the Japanese version of instrument for measuring self-regulation was developed based on well-established measures, further examination may be warranted too establish its validity.

Response to the comment 3

Thank you for the suggestion. We have revised the title as suggested by the comment, and removed all the terms ‘shaper’ from the manuscript.

Response to the comment 4

Thank you for the comment. As suggested by the comment 1, we have revised the manuscript to define hopefulness as a covariate.

We wish to thank the reviewers again for the valuable comments.

Submitted filename: RevisionNotesR1_PONE-D-19-16626.docx

Decision Letter 1

PONE-D-19-16626R1

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ , click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at [email protected] .

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact [email protected] .

With kind regards,

Acceptance letter

11 Sep 2019

Dear Dr. Nakanishi:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact [email protected] .

For any other questions or concerns, please email [email protected] .

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Geilson Lima Santana

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data availability statement.

  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (492.9 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

IMAGES

  1. Role Model Research Paper Assignment and Guideline by Ms Annas Shop of

    research paper on role model

  2. Role Model Descriptive And Narrative Essay Examples

    research paper on role model

  3. Role Model Essay

    research paper on role model

  4. Writing an Essay On Role Models: Principles To Keep in Mind

    research paper on role model

  5. My Role Model Example: [Essay Example], 597 words

    research paper on role model

  6. SOLUTION: Role model personal reflection paper

    research paper on role model