U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • v.2016; Nov-Dec 2016

Understanding the Terrorist Mind

Editor’s note:.

While early research focused on the political roots of terrorism, many of today’s investigators are probing the psychological factors that drive adherents to commit their deadly deeds. Are terrorists mentally ill or do they rationally weigh the costs and benefits of their actions and conclude that terrorism is profitable? Our author traces recent advances in using imaging and experimental research to determine what motivates monstrous acts .

In the past 15 years, dramatic acts of terror have been committed against citizens of many countries. A reasonable first step towards addressing such violence is understanding where it comes from—what motivates people to join terror organizations and engage in terrorism. Recent work in experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience provides some perspective on the mind of a terrorist.

First, it is useful to define what we mean by ‘terrorism.’ When Americans and Europeans think of terrorists, they likely imagine Muslim extremists. For example, Google searches for the term ‘terrorist’ in the month after the Boston Marathon bombings and Paris attacks (committed by Muslim extremists) increased three-fold and six-fold, respectively, relative to the months prior to the attacks. By contrast, similarly deadly attacks by Anders Breivik in Norway, who killed 69 children, and Dylann Roof, who killed nine black parishioners in the American south, were not followed by an increase in such searches. In fact, compared to the three months leading up to it, there was a slight decrease in searches using the term ‘terrorist’ after the Breivik attack.

Although it may be comforting to think of terrorists as people unlike us, I will argue that this belief belies an uncomfortable reality: that the psychological processes that drive an individual to engage in terrorism are deeply human, common across cultures—and traits that likely reside in us all.

The definition of terrorism that I use here will include a two key elements. First, it involves a group ideology. Individuals may attack, threaten, terrorize or kill others, but if they are not part of a group and not motivated to do so by an ideology, then by this definition they are not terrorists. Even a violent group, such as a drug cartel that beheads civilians, would not be considered a terrorist organization, since its members are not ideologically motivated (note, however, that many of the processes that I describe below also apply to gangs). Second, terrorism is defined by the use of violence in the service of the group’s ideology, and particularly violence that indiscriminately targets members of a group (e.g., civilians, children). Many governments would challenge this point, as they have convicted non-violent Native and environmental activists who sabotage logging equipment under anti-terrorism laws. However, I do not consider these groups to be composed of ‘terrorists’ (but again, many of the processes described below still apply).

In the context of this definition, I will try to offer some insight into the mind of a terrorist by looking at what lies in the human mind more generally. Specifically, I will seek to explain why and how individuals support or engage in ‘indiscriminate violence driven by group ideologies’ by looking at our understanding of three processes: (1) how our brains respond to groups, (2) how our brains are led to condone or initiate acts of indiscriminate violence, and (3) how our brains process ideological information.

Social Factors

For most of the millions of years that our species has been around, humans have eked out an existence only through the coordinated effort of small, cohesive coalitions. Evolution has therefore shaped within us a deep desire to belong to groups. In modern times, social belonging remains a major psychological need, which we fill by connecting with others through a variety of ‘social identities’–Californian, professor, rugby player, progressive, vegetarian, for example. We each contain multitudes. The tendency to connect through one of these multitudes can be reflexive (“Oh wow, you’re from California, too?”).

From this perspective, the appeal of ‘terrorist groups’ is completely unremarkable. Just as a fraternity, team, club, military unit, or gang can provide a deep social connection with others, so too can ISIS, Al-Qaeda, or white, nationalist groups. Many think that people join groups for what they do (terrorists join terror groups because they are violent people; men join fraternities because they drink and party), but the deep, fundamental motivation to join any group is the need to socially connect. From this view, individuals most at risk for joining a terrorist group are not those who are poor or violent, but those who are alienated and thus drawn to an arrangement that can offer the camaraderie, brotherhood, and purpose that they are missing. This may help explain why very different demographics—the young man at a refugee camp who is deprived of regional, professional, and academic identities, and the middle-class child of immigrants in a Western country who feels alienated from his or her host country—are common recruits for ISIS. And perhaps other groups. It may also explain why regular attendance at a mosque—which provides a strong social identity—is inversely correlated with ISIS enrollment.

Although social identities can in themselves provide clear paths to bring individuals together, the brain seems particularly prone not only to creating an ‘us,’ but also readily defining a ‘them.’ A classic study that demonstrated the ease with which group identities arise came from a team of experimental psychologists in the 1950s, led by Muzafer Sherif. In the study, the researchers aimed to generate and dissipate group conflict in a set of middle class white boys who attended a camp set up by the researchers at a park in Oklahoma. The plan was to separate the boys into two groups and then organize a series of activities to establish competitive group identities. In fact, the participants pre-empted the researchers’ strategy: the boys caught wind of each other, and immediately formed their own group identities (the ‘Eagles’ and the ‘Rattlers’) and started competing on their own—staking territory, raiding cabins, and picking fights. The silver lining of the study came when the researchers demonstrated how readily they could undo the group distinctions that they had facilitated. By orchestrating a series of threats to the entire camp (a ‘broken’ well, a stuck van) that could only be solved by working together, the stark distinctions between Eagles and Rattlers began to fade as they all adopted the overarching identity of ‘campers.’

Inspired in part by the Sherif study, a host of experimental studies have demonstrated the ease with which people start thinking in terms of ‘us’ and ‘them,’ and the consequences of such distinctions. 1 For example, this research has found in controlled lab environments that people assigned to groups based on arbitrary distinctions (e.g. whether they are ‘underestimators’ or ‘overestimators’ of the number of dots on a screen) perceive members of their group to be more intelligent, trustworthy, and attractive than those from the other group. Even if people are explicitly told that the groups are arbitrarily assigned, their minds lead them to assign in-group members higher value than outgroup members.

The Impact of Imaging

With the advent of neuroimaging, we have begun to access the inner workings of some of these group-based processes. This has been particularly important, since the mental events associated with ‘us’ versus ‘them’ thinking are likely unconscious, and therefore difficult to assess through self-report. For example, a number of research groups have demonstrated an ‘out-group race face’ bias in a brain: when white participants see pictures of black versus white faces, they register more activity in the amygdala, a brain region that drives fear learning. 2 Since the amount of neural bias is unassociated with explicit anti-black attitudes, this has been taken as evidence that when confronted with a black American, white Americans experience an automatic fear response that they are unaware of and do not necessarily condone. 3

A particularly interesting iteration of this in-group/out-group face bias illustrates the malleability of in-group/out-group distinctions: Jay van Bavel and colleagues showed that the bias in amygdala activity among White participants was also present more towards an arbitrarily defined outgroup versus the arbitrarily defined in-group, even if the groups were mixed-race. 4 That is, among white Americans, there was more activity in the amygdala when viewing black versus white faces, but when the same faces were assigned to mixed-race teams, the amygdala now responded more strongly to faces from the ‘out-group’ than they did to faces from the ‘in-group,’ regardless of race. We not only have a tendency to generate ‘us’ and ‘them’, but who qualifies for each can be completely flexible, and race/skin color is just one of many arbitrary dimensions over which people can be categorized.

A study published just recently examined the neural basis of another psychological process that has been shown to be distributed parochially: trust. Here, Zaki and colleagues had participants play trust-based economic investment games with in-group and out-group members—in this case, own or rival school members. 5 They found that trusting in-group members resulted in more activity in brain regions associated with pleasure, while trusting outgroup members resulted in more activity in brain regions normally associated with cognitive effort (e.g., consciously withholding a response that you desperately want to give, or re-assessing a situation). The implication here is that in-group trust comes easy, while out-group trust comes only with effort.

Together, such psychology and imaging studies give us some insight into our genetic legacy. We have inherited brains that are inherently sensitive to group affiliation. We find meaning in our lives through social identities, and we experience comfort with those who share these identities. However, when creating an ‘us,’ the brain seems to seek out a ‘them,’ bringing online a series of psychological processes—including fear and distrust—which colors our view of outgroup members.

Although this schematic helps to illuminate some of the underlying dynamics that may drive people to join a terror, ISIS and groups like it are not merely fraternities or clubs. Terror groups also have an explicit ideology, and membership carries with it a tacit willingness to kill civilians. What neural and psychological processes help us understand the willingness to attach to ideologies, and to condone violence?

The Brain on Ideology

One of the most striking characteristics of terrorist groups is their strict adherence to an ideology. Ideologies provide a narrative structure with which to interpret new information and past events. Since terror groups (without exception, I believe) are composed of an aggrieved minority, their ideology is often centered around a narrative of victimhood.

Such narratives seem particularly powerful; especially, perhaps, for parochial altruists—people who love their own group so much that they are willing to die on its behalf. If you perceive that your group’s back is against the wall, this might be just the thing to motivate a parochial altruist to act on their behalf. 6 Perhaps this is why we see the narrative of victimhood even among some of the most powerful groups in the world. For example, note that the ‘don’t tread on me’ American flag is still widely visible in the US. In fact, groups often compete with each other for who is the aggrieved victim in a conflict (i.e., ‘competitive victimhood’), which buys them more third-party support, but also may motivate their members to action. 7

Whether about victimhood or not, ideologies are incredibly persistent. Part of what gives them their momentum is a set of cognitive filters that help process incoming information to support and enhance the in-group’s ideological narrative. For example, confirmation bias describes the tendency to uncritically accept information that confirms their group’s beliefs, and scrutinize anything that runs counter to their ideological leaning. Certainly, anyone who has paid any attention to the current US election cycle has seen this at play. Another critical bias concerns the way that we construe the deviant actions of others. If I find myself doing something wrong (e.g., cutting late into a merging lane), it is easy for me to justify this by external circumstances (e.g., “I was late for an important meeting”). But when I see others doing the same, I tend to attribute this to their internal characteristics (e.g., “they are selfish jerks”). As was famously expressed by the comedian George Carlin, “Have you ever noticed that anybody driving slower than you is a moron, and everybody driving faster than you is a maniac?” The inter-group context only magnifies this process— their violence reflects ‘who they are’ (barbarians, colonizers, terrorists), whereas our violence is shrouded in circumstance (“we had to kill them because…”).

The neural infrastructure built up around maintaining ideological righteousness is immense. Dozens of distinct biases have been identified, named, and characterized. 8 And since these processes occur automatically, in regions of our brain that are generally inaccessible to conscious introspection, we are subject to their effects whether we like it or not. We are, as the great psychologist Lee Ross said, “naïve realists” who believe that we alone see the world objectively, whereas those who disagree with us are inherently irrational. This ‘bias blind spot’ is again not owned by some groups and not others—they are part of a consequence of having a human brain that is designed to operate efficiently.

The Brain on Violence

Finally, a hallmark of terrorism, in my definition, is indiscriminate violence against members of the ‘out-group.’ Inter-group violence is by no means limited to terrorist groups: established governments and nation states have been responsible for the deaths of hundreds of thousands of people over the past decade. How different is ‘their’ violence from ‘ours’?

Our brains are shaped with the capability to care deeply, but also to kill. This deep ambivalence is potentially problematic. A society filled with people who are inherently very compassionate and very violent might prove unstable. Part of evolution’s solution to this problem seems to have been to tether the processes that undergird pro-sociality (e.g., empathy) and the processes that enable violence (e.g., dehumanization) to in-group and out-group distinctions. In this way, people would be potentiated to love the in-group and hate the outgroup; to fight and die on behalf of ‘us’ and to be willing to kill ‘them.’ The psychological processes that drive deep altruism (for the in-group) and motivate extreme violence (towards the outgroup) still live within us.

In the US, we fight others by proxy with our professional militaries, and so we are rarely put in a situation that would involve directly harming out-group members. But the willingness to harm others can still be assessed among non-military citizens of western democracies. Since experimental evidence is scant from actual members of terror groups, I will provide the evidence for group-based violence from ‘us’ – members of mostly majority groups that have the potential to act violently on our behalf. I will argue that these processes are similar to those acting in terrorists who actually pull the trigger.

So what drives someone to commit political violence (ideologically motivated violence), more generally? I find it useful to think of the psychology of political violence as a collection of impulses within us that tug us either towards or away from violence. If the various pulls towards violence are strong enough and the pulls away from it weak enough, a person engages in political violence; if not, they don’t. Below is a brief outline of work I’ve done to illuminate two of the processes contributing to this psychological calculus: empathy and dehumanization.

The Empathy Factor

We are accustomed to thinking of empathy as an unambiguous force for social good. And for sound reasons–empathy is a “social glue” that arguably has been fundamental in enabling large groups of unrelated humans to band together in complex, cooperative societies. Although good experimental evidence shows that the amount of empathy one possesses (i.e., trait empathy) or expresses (i.e., state empathy) can drive altruism, there is also reason to believe that empathy may not be as unambiguously pro-social in inter-group contexts. 9 , 10 Specifically, whereas empathy for an outgroup likely motivates pro-sociality towards its members, in-group empathy may have the opposite effect: if people feel the suffering of in-group members particularly acutely, this may motivate them to act against members of an out-group that they see as responsible.

In experimental research, I have tested the effects of in-group empathy and out-group empathy (and the difference between the two, i.e., ‘parochial empathy’) in three contexts: Americans regarding Arabs, Greeks regarding Germans (during the Greek financial crisis), and Hungarians regarding Muslim refugees (during the refugee crisis). Predictably, in all of these settings, the more empathy participants reported feeling for the suffering of random outgroup members, the greater their willingness to help and the less their willingness to harm needy members of that group (e.g., donations to civilian victims of drone strikes). However, empathy for in-group suffering predicted the opposite: less willingness to help the outgroup and more willingness to harm. In fact, this is the conclusion drawn by a number of researchers who have interviewed attempted suicide bombers or families of people who had engaged in suicide bombings. Although some who commit political violence appear to be unhampered by empathy, the majority tend to be characterized by a strong communal focus that includes compassion and caring for others. 11

Empathy therefore contributes two ropes to the internal tug-of-war: the greater the pull from in-group empathy to harm the out-group, and the weaker the pull from out-group empathy to prevent this, the stronger the overall motivation to engage in or condone intergroup aggression. It is therefore the difference in empathy, rather than the capacity for empathy, that best predicts intergroup violence.

The Dehumanization Factor

Historically, dehumanization has accompanied some of the darkest chapters in human history. During colonization, slavery, genocide, and war, depictions of the other side as uncivilized brutes or animals has been commonplace. We see this type of dehumanizing rhetoric from terror groups today—not only are we, the ‘infidels,’ referred to as ‘pigs’ or ‘dogs,’ but we are viewed as undifferentiated and therefore collectively responsible. The rhetoric in western democracies about disliked Muslim groups and terrorists is nearly identical: Iranians, Hamas and ISIS have been depicted in the mainstream media as rats, beasts, snakes or vermin in need of extermination.

In recent work, I have attempted to go beyond current psychological trends that use subtle measures of ‘everyday dehumanization’ to capture overt expressions of dehumanization that were typical of colonial times and still seem present today. Toward this end, we developed a measure that captures blatant dehumanization using the popular ‘Ascent of Man’ diagram, which depicts evolutionary ‘progress’ with five images, from a quadrupedal early human ancestor through fully upright ‘modern man.’ By asking people to indicate where on the image certain groups fall, we have been able to assess levels of perceived ‘humanity’ among a range of participant groups, towards a host of targets. 12

To our academic delight (and personal dismay), we have found that people from every country we have assayed (the US., England, Denmark, the Netherlands, Spain, Greece, Hungary, Israel, Palestine, and Jordan, and the state of Palestine) rate at least one other group to be at least 15 points lower on the 100-point Ascent dehumanization scale than their own.

At the individual level, ratings of Ascent dehumanization are highly consequential. In Europe, for example, the degree to which people dehumanize Muslim refugees predicts their support for anti-refugee policies and resistance to refugee settlement, even when accounting for conservatism and prejudice. 13 In the US, levels of Ascent dehumanization are associated with positions on a range of socially relevant issues, including willingness to sign petitions opposing the Iranian Nuclear Accord. In a recent study inspired by anti-Muslim rhetoric from the Presidential campaign, we found that the dehumanization of Muslims was strongly associated with the willingness to punish all Muslims for individual acts of terrorism. 14

What’s more, the consequences of dehumanization go beyond how they motivate members of the dehumanizing group – they also affect the dehumanized. Specifically, we found that the more dehumanized Muslim Americans feel , the more likely they are to dehumanize Americans, which leads to greater support for violent forms of collective action (i.e., ‘by any means necessary)’, and less willingness to report suspicious activity in their communities to the FBI. 15 Making others feel dehumanized therefore puts us all at greater risk of that group allowing violence to happen, which could be interepreted by the dehumanizing group to justify (and compound) their dehumanization. Of course, the interaction between meta-dehumanization, dehumanization and support for violence can easily ratchet up intergroup conflicts.

Thus, if dehumanization cuts a psychological thread that normally inhibits intergroup aggression, ‘meta-dehumanization’ can provide the scissors. It is certainly not much of a stretch to imagine that terrorists think that they are dehumanized by westerners. In fact, gien the prevalence of dehumanizing depictions and language used to describe terrorists, it would be shocking if they did not.

Pondering the Future

While we like to think of ‘terrorists’ as sociopaths and misfits distinct from ‘us’ and united with each other by shared pathology and unfettered hatred, in fact their most salient characteristics—fervent attachment to a group ideology and a willingness to engage in indiscriminate violence—are likely driven by deep psychological processes shaped in the human mind through evolution. The great irony, then, may be that the best way to understand the mind of a terrorist is by examining our own.

Much of how we view terrorists is built upon a series of assumptions. Primary among these is that ‘they started it.’ Afghanistan and Iraq were a response to 9/11. But from terror groups’ perspective, they are the aggrieved party and it is the other side that started it. An insightful observation recently came from a special-forces lieutenant who related overhearing some of his men talking about the Taliban they were fighting in Afghanistan. Referring to the classic patriotic film Red Dawn , where a group of rural Americans fight off the invading Russian army, one of the American soldiers said to the other: “If this is Red Dawn , we’re the Russians.” If we accept that they are right and we attacked them first (even if only in their own minds), then how differently do we imagine we would behave in their situation?

The insightful comments from the soldiers quoted above notwithstanding, the reality is that we are using stone age psychology to solve 21 st century conflicts. But there is hope. As much as evolution has baked into the human brain psychological processes that lead us stumbling into conflict, that same brain is endowed with an overriding organizational principle: flexibility. As powerful as these destructive unconscious forces may be, we are built to be able to gain conscious control of them. For example, our recent work has shown that if people are made aware of the hypocrisy of holding Muslims as a group responsible for terror attacks without considering white people similarly responsible for violence committed by white supremacists, their collective blame of Muslims dramatically decreases, which then ameliorates their endorsement of violence against Muslims. 16 Determining which of the unconscious biases that underlie our ideological certainty can be inoculated against is one step towards mastering the destructive tendencies in our own minds.

One of the great gifts that science has given to humanity over the last 2000 years is humility. The Earth is not the center of the universe. Our DNA is not fundamentally different from that of other living things. And our brains do not differ markedly from those of feared of hated others. The great hope from the neuroscience revolution is that awareness of our own brains may actually allow us to transcend the unconscious processes that drive us to conflict.

Emile Bruneau , Ph.D., is a researcher and lecturer at the Annenberg School for Communication at the University of Pennsylvania. Prior to his formal training in neuroscience, Bruneau worked, traveled, and lived in a number of conflict regions: South Africa during the transition from Apartheid to Democracy, Sri Lanka during one of the largest Tamil Tiger strikes in that nation’s history, Ireland during “The Troubles,” and Israel/Palestine around the Second Intifada. Bruneau is now working to bring the tools of science to bear on the problem of intergroup conflict by characterizing the (often unconscious) cognitive biases that drive conflict, and critically evaluating efforts aimed at transcending these biases. In 2015, he received a Bok Center Award for teaching at Harvard, and was honored with the Ed Cairns Early Career Award in Peace Psychology. His work has received funding from the UN, US Institute for Peace, Soros Foundation, DARPA, ONR, and DRAPER Laboratories. Bruneau received his doctorate from the University of Michigan.

Psychology of terrorism: Introduction to the special issue

Affiliation.

  • 1 Georgia State University.
  • PMID: 28383973
  • DOI: 10.1037/amp0000148

Despite the extraordinary social and political consequences often associated with terrorist violence, as well as our responses to it, psychological research on terrorist behavior is conspicuously underdeveloped. This special issue of American Psychologist presents a series of articles that showcase new conceptual, theoretical, and empirical advances in our understanding of terrorism. In doing so, it seeks to not merely summarize recent accomplishments, but to highlight the immense value of explicitly psychological research on these issues, far more of which is called for to realize the potential for informing solutions. (PsycINFO Database Record

(c) 2017 APA, all rights reserved).

Publication types

  • Introductory Journal Article
  • Terrorism / psychology*
  • Search Menu
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Lifestyle, Home, and Garden
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business History
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Developmental and Physical Disabilities Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

Psychology of Terrorism

Psychology of Terrorism

Psychology of Terrorism

Calvin Professor of Psychology, Pacific School of Psychology and Consulting Professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

William McInnes Distinguished Professor of Psychology

Associate Director, Stanford Center for Interdisciplinary Policy, Research and Education on Terrorism; Professor of Psychology and Director of Training, PCSP-Stanford Psy.D. Consortium

Professor of Psychology

  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

The past decade has witnessed a dramatic transformation in the nature and use of political terrorism. Psychology of Terrorism is an indispensable resource in the field of psychology and terrorism. Given the relative newness of the discipline, there is a great need for a text that covers relevant aspects of psychology related to terrorism. This online resource presents the newest findings on treatment and clinical response protocols. Other topics included the theory and history of terrorism, types and effects of weapons of mass destruction or disruption, the role of nation, state, and local agencies and volunteer groups responding to terrorist threat, military response, psychological consequences of terrorism, special populations, prevention, training, and research. This guide fills the need for a single, comprehensive resource for mental health clinicians, medical care providers, researchers, educators, and others who respond to acts of terrorism.

Signed in as

Institutional accounts.

  • Google Scholar Indexing
  • GoogleCrawler [DO NOT DELETE]

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Sign in through your institution

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Kruglanski & Fishman (2006) The Psychology of Terrorism- "Syndrome" versus "Tool" Perspectives

Profile image of arie kruglanski

Related Papers

arie kruglanski

psychology of terrorism research paper

Luis De la corte

Research approaches leading to the understanding of the terrorist phenomenon are suggested. Seven principles explaining terrorism are analysed from a psychosocial approach: They could complete the explanations of terrorism focused in socio-structural and psychological variables, the latter being insufficient and often erroneous. Thus, terrorism is presented as a strategy of political influence, and the psychological attributes of terrorists are explained as the result of several influential social processes. We considered that terrorist attacks and campaigns correspond to strategic reasons, but we also assume that the rationality of the terrorists perspective is limited. As far as the organisational parameters are concerned, we propose an analogy with ordinary social movements, especially relating to their ideological frameworks of legitimisation and resource mobilization processes.

Myths, Reality and Ways Forward

Dipak Gupta

Sebastian Wojciechowski

Dr RUKUNDO Levi, PhD

Ami Pedahzur

A conceptual framework is presented for addressing psychological issues in the development of the terrorist. In particular, the authors suggest that viewing terror- ism as a process may lead not only to better conceptual development in analyses of the terrorist, but may also lead to the development of more practical and efficient counterterrorism initiatives. Additionally, viewing terrorism in this way may finally lead to the formation of a clear and unambiguous position for psychology within an interdisciplinary approach to analyses of both terrorism and the terrorist.

Studies in Conflict & Terrorism

Alice LoCicero

Randy Borum

This report analyzes and synthesizes what has been reported from the scientific and professional literature about the psychology of terrorism. By attempting to identify, describe, and evaluate what contribution psychological theory or research may have made to understanding terrorists and terrorism. One of the gravest threats to the national security environment is without question, terrorism. In order to better understand the causes, motivations, and determinants of terrorist behavior, this report identifies and analyzes the scientific and professional social science literature pertaining to the psychological and/or behavioral dimensions of terrorist behavior. The objective is to explore what questions pertaining to terrorist groups and behavior had been asked by social science researchers, to identify the main findings from that research, and attempt to distill and summarize them within a framework of operationally relevant questions. Highlights of key findings on the psychology of terrorism include: (1) people become terrorists in different ways, in different roles, and for different reasons; (2) mental illness is not a critical factor in explaining terrorist behaviors and most terrorists are not psychopaths; (3) histories of childhood abuse and trauma and themes of perceived injustice and humiliation often are prominent in terrorist biographies, but do not help to explain terrorism; (4) not all extremist ideologies promote violence, nor are all extremists violent; (5) terrorist groups have certain internal and external vulnerabilities to their existence; (6) little research has been conducted on terrorist recruitment; (7) effective leaders of terrorist organizations must be able to maintain a collective belief system, establish and maintain organizational routines, control the flow of communication, deflect conflict to external targets, manipulate incentives for followers, and keep action going; and (8) research on the psychology of terrorism largely lacks substance and rigor. References

RELATED PAPERS

Pages 154–204 in The Betrayal of the Humanities: The University during the Third Reich, edited by Bernard M. Levinson and Robert P. Ericksen. Studies in Antisemitism. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, 2022.

Bernard M Levinson

Mariana Vazquez

Revue des études juives

Thierry MURCIA

Katia Antonieta

Adriana Vlasceanu

Oxford Bibliographies in Latin American Studies

Manny Medrano

Alicia Ottenwalder

Introduction to Material Engineering Lecture 1

Mohammad A B U B A K A R Atiq

INTELLIGENCE IN ISLAM

Zin Eddine Dadach

İbrahim Cicioglu

remeena shaji

Revue d’économie industrielle

Rachel Guillain

Salud Pública de México

Eduardo de la Garza

Dave Richers

Gastroentérologie Clinique et Biologique

Lamia Kallel

Educación Química

Margarita Flores Zepeda

Kelman Wieder

Journal of Hepatology

Anna Fernandez

Research Journal of Finance and Accounting

gregory Lyimo

John Deva Prasanna

Journal of Applied Physics

Chien-Cheng Kuo

Oceanography

Charles Hussey

Asian Journal of Pharmaceutical and Clinical Research

Anitha Jegadeeshwari L

Italian journal of anatomy and embryology

Craniomaxillofacial Trauma & Reconstruction

JOSE LUIS DEL CASTILLO PARDO DE VERA

Innovative Materials and Technologies in Metallurgy and Mechanical Engineering

Gulmira Yar-Mukhamedova

Journal of Numerical Cognition

İlyas KARADENİZ

USYD毕业证成绩单办理悉尼大学假文凭定制 QQ微信1989 88881

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Impact of terrorism on child sex at birth: evidence from Pakistan

  • Published: 14 May 2024

Cite this article

psychology of terrorism research paper

  • Khusrav Gaibulloev 1 ,
  • Gerel Oyun 2 &
  • Javed Younas   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2032-2448 1  

20 Accesses

Explore all metrics

Using insights from the literature on psychology and medicine, we examine the impact of stress induced by terrorism on child sex at birth. The psychological and social stressors associated with terrorist events prior to conception may trigger changes in parental hormones that have an implication for birth outcomes. We extract data on 11,331 live births conceived between 2007 and 2012 from Pakistan Demographic and Health Survey 2012–2013 and match these data with household information, monthly terrorist incidents at home district, and other district-level characteristics. Our analysis shows that parental exposure to terrorism prior to conception reduces the likelihood of a male birth. We examine the birth outcome of siblings by exploiting the variation in exposure to terrorism across pregnancies for a given mother and confirm our finding. The results provide microeconomic evidence of the potential long-term impact of terrorism on population dynamics and development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

psychology of terrorism research paper

The New Roles of Men and Women and Implications for Families and Societies

psychology of terrorism research paper

Gender inequality as a barrier to economic growth: a review of the theoretical literature

psychology of terrorism research paper

Gender Perspective of Victimization, Crime and Penal Policy

Lee ( 2014 ) finds that cohort of children born to mothers exposed to the trauma caused by the Korean War (1950–1953) had much worse levels of health, education, and labor market outcomes during their lifetime. Interestingly, women's health outcomes were unaffected by the war, whereas men born in the 1950 saw higher rates of disability and mortality at older ages.

Valente ( 2015 ) focuses on exposure to conflict. She does not find a statistically significant relationship between conflict and the neonatal mortality or child size at birth.

Studies on the Mexican Drug Wars (Brown 2018 ) and homicides in Brazil (Koppensteiner and Manacorda 2016 ) reveal that in-utero exposure to these disturbances in the residence areas of mothers leads to a substantial decrease in the birthweight of newborn. Employing heterogeneous analysis, both studies find that the low birthweight effect is more prominent in children of mothers with low levels of education. This implies that violence exacerbates the disadvantage that these children experience as a result of the lower socioeconomic status of their households.

This body of literature is large (e.g., Abadie and Gardeazabal 2003 , 2008 ; Arce 2018 ; Bandyopadhyay, Sandler, and Younas 2018 , 2014 ; Chesney, Reshetar, and Karaman 2011 ; Drakos and Kutan 2003 , Gaibulloev and Sandler 2008 , 2009 , 2011 ; Gaibulloev, Sandler, and Sul 2014 , Younas 2015 ). See Gaibulloev and Sandler ( 2019 ) for a recent comprehensive review of terrorism literature.

Sen ( 1990 ) provides a shocking analysis that fomented a large body of research on gender bias against women. He pointed out that 100 million missing girls should have been born, but they simply never existed likely due to the strong preference for boys and abortion of female fetuses.

Evidence from the great East Japan earthquake of 2011 shows that stress may increase the rate of male fetal losses in utero and also reduce the odds of male conception (Catalano, Yorifuji and Kawachi 2013 ).

Primary sex ratio refers to the ratio of males to females at the time of conception, when the sperm fertilizes the egg, and secondary sex ratio refers to the male-to-female ratio at the time of childbirth.

The reason is that cervical mucus viscosity varies across the cycle with being thick immediately before and after ovulation, thus giving advantage to smaller and shorter Y-bearing sperm to reach and fuse with an egg (Jongbloet et al. 2001 ).

The National Institute of Population Studies (NIPS) conducted the survey under the guidance of the Ministry of National Health Services, Regulations and Coordination of Pakistan. Sampling is based on a stratified two-stage cluster sampling procedure. The population is stratified into urban and rural area frames. At the first stage, clusters of about 200 to 250 households are identified within each sampling frame, and 498 clusters in total are selected to represent the population of the four provinces of Pakistan and Gilgit Baltistan region. The sample excludes Azad Jammu and Kashmir, FATA, and restricted military and protected areas. At the second stage, 28 households are randomly chosen for interviews from each cluster by applying a systematic sampling technique with a random start.

We exclude all events that are coded as terrorism. The aggregate (non-terror) political violence—in addition to violences captured in the five listed variables—includes threat of violence and other types of violence that are not easily categorized.

Abadie A, Gardeazabal J (2003) The economic costs of conflict: a case study of the Basque Country. Am Econ Rev 93(1):113–132

Article   Google Scholar  

Abadie A, Gardeazabal J (2008) Terrorism and the world economy. Eur Econ Rev 52(1):1–27

Almond D, Currie J (2011) Killing me softly: the fetal origins hypothesis. J Econ Perspect 25(3):153–172

Arce DG (2018) On the human consequences of terrorism. Public Choice Forthcoming 178:371

Bandyopadhyay S, Sandler T, Younas J (2014) Foreign direct investment, aid, and terrorism. Oxf Econ Pap 66(1):25–50

Bandyopadhyay S, Sandler T, Younas J (2018) Trade and terrorism: a disaggregated approach. J Peace Res 55(5):656–670

Becker GS, Rubinstein Y (2004) Fear and the response to terrorism: an economic analysis, University of Chicago mimeo, p 93

Beratis NG, Asimacopoulou A, Varvarigou A (2008) Association of secondary sex ratio with smoking and parity. Fertil Steril 89(3):662–667

Berrebi C, Ostwald J (2014) Terrorism and fertility: evidence for a causal influence of terrorism on fertility. Oxf Econ Pap 67(1):63–82

Brainerd E (2017) The lasting effect of sex ratio imbalance on marriage and family: evidence from World War II in Russia. Rev Econ Stat 99(2):229–242

Brown R (2018) The Mexican drug war and early-life health: the impact of violent crime on birth outcomes. Demography 55(1):319–340

Bruckner TA, Catalano R, Ahern J (2010) Male fetal loss in the US following the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001. BMC Public Health 10(1):273–278

Burlando A (2014) Transitory shocks and birth weights: evidence from a blackout in Zanzibar. J Dev Econ 108:154–168

Cagnacci A, Renzi A, Arangino S, Alessandrini C, Volpe A (2005) Interplay between maternal weight and seasons in determining the secondary sex ratio of human offspring. Fertil Steril 84(1):246–248

Camacho A (2008) Stress and birth weight: evidence from terrorist attacks. Am Econ Rev 98(2):511–515

Cameron EZ, Lemons PR, Bateman PW, Bennett NC (2008) Experimental alteration of litter sex ratios in a mammal. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 275(1632):323–327

Carlson K (2015) Fear itself: the effects of distressing economic news on birth outcomes. J Health Econ 41:117–132

Catalano RA (2003) Sex ratios in the two Germanies: a test of the economic stress hypothesis. Hum Reprod 18(9):1972–1975

Catalano R, Yorifuji T, Kawachi I (2013) Natural selection in utero: evidence from the Great East Japan Earthquake. Am J Hum Biol 25(4):555–559

Chahnazarian A (1988) Determinants of the sex ratio at birth: review of recent literature. Soc Biol 35(3–4):214–235

Google Scholar  

Chason RJ, McLain AC, Sundaram R, Chen Z, Segars JH, Pyper C, Louis GMB (2012) Preconception stress and the secondary sex ratio: A prospective cohort study. Fertil Steril 98(4):937–941

Chesney M, Reshetar G, Karaman M (2011) The impact of terrorism on financial markets: an empirical study. J Bank Financ 35(2):253–267

Clifton VL (2010) Sex and the human placenta: mediating differential strategies of fetal growth and survival. Placenta 31:S33–S39

Coale AJ (1991) Excess female mortality and the balance of the sexes in the population: an estimate of the number of "missing females. Popul Dev Rev 17(3):517–523

de Mesquita B, Ethan CC, Fair JJ, Rais RB, Shapiro JN (2015) Measuring political violence in Pakistan: insights from the BFRS Dataset. Confl Manag Peace Sci 32(5):536–558

Drakos K, Kutan AM (2003) Regional effects of terrorism on tourism in three Mediterranean countries. J Conflict Resolut 47(5):621–641

Enders W, Sandler T, Gaibulloev K (2011) Domestic versus transnational terrorism: data, decomposition, and dynamics. J Peace Res 48(3):319–337

Eskenazi B, Marks AR, Catalano R, Bruckner T, Toniolo PG (2007) Low birthweight in New York City and upstate New York following the events of September 11th. Hum Reprod 22(11):3013–3020

Fareed M, Ahmad MK, Anwar MA, Afzal M (2017) Impact of consanguineous marriages and degrees of inbreeding on fertility, child mortality, secondary sex ratio, selection intensity, and genetic load: a cross-sectional study from Northern India. Pediatr Res 81(1):18–26

Frey BS, Luechinger S, Stutzer A (2009) The life satisfaction approach to valuing public goods: the case of terrorism. Public Choice 138(3–4):317–345

Fukuda M, Fukuda K, Shimizu T, Yomura W, Shimizu S (1996) Kobe earthquake and reduced sperm motility. Hum Reprod 11(6):1244–1246

Fukuda M, Fukuda K, Shimizu T, Møller H (1998) Decline in sex ratio at birth after Kobe earthquake. Hum Reprod 13(8):2321–2322

Gaibulloev K, Sandler T (2008) Growth consequences of terrorism in Western Europe. Kyklos 61(3):411–424.

Gaibulloev K, Sandler T (2009) The impact of terrorism and conflicts on growth in Asia. Economics & Politics 21(3):359–383

Gaibulloev K, Sandler T (2011) The adverse effect of transnational and domestic terrorism on growth in Africa. J Peace Res 48(3):355–371.

Gaibulloev K, Sandler T (2019) What we have learned about terrorism since 9/11. J Econ Lit 57(2):275–328

Gaibulloev K, Sandler T, Sul D (2014) Dynamic panel analysis under cross-sectional dependence. Polit Anal 22(2):258–273

Gaibulloev K, Oyun G, Younas J (2019) Terrorism and subjective financial well-being: Micro-level evidence from Pakistan. Public Choice 178(3):493–512

Jacobsen R, Møller H, Mouritsen A (1999) Natural variation in the human sex ratio. Hum Reprod 14(12):3120–3125

James WH, Grech V (2017) A review of the established and suspected causes of variations in human sex ratio at birth. Early Human Dev 109:50–56

Jongbloet PH, Zielhuis GA, Groenewoud HM, Pasker-De Jong PC (2001) The secular trends in male:female ratio at birth in postwar industrialized countries. Environ Health Perspect 109(7):749–752

Klimek M, Pabian W, Tomaszewska B, Kołodziejczyk J (2005) Levels of plasma ACTH in men from infertile couples. Neuro-Endocrinol Lett 26(4):347–350

Koppensteiner MF, Manacorda M (2016) Violence and birth outcomes: evidence from homicides in Brazil. J Dev Econ 119:16–33

Kraemer S (2000) The fragile male. BMJ 321(7276):1609–1612

Lauderdale DS (2006) Birth outcomes for Arabic-named women in California before and after September 11. Demography 43(1):185–201

Lee C (2014) In utero exposure to the Korean War and its long-term effects on socioeconomic and health outcomes. J Health Econ 33:76–93

Mansour H, Rees DI (2012) Armed conflict and birth weight: evidence from the al-Aqsa Intifada. J Dev Econ 99(1):190–199

Marshall RD, Bryant RA, Amsel L, Suh EJ, Cook JM, Neria Y (2007) The psychology of ongoing threat: relative risk appraisal, the September 11 attacks, and terrorism-related fears. Am Psychol 62(4):304–316

Mathews F, Johnson PJ, Neil A (2008) You are what your mother eats: evidence for maternal preconception diet influencing foetal sex in humans. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 275(1643):1661–1668

Nasir M (2021) Prenatal exposure to shocks and early-life health: impact of terrorism and flood on birth outcomes in Pakistan. Def Peace Econ 32(5):572–587

National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism (START) (2015) Global terrorism database (GTD). www.start.umd.edu/gtd . Accessed 25 Aug 2017

Navara KJ (2010) Programming of offspring sex ratios by maternal stress in humans: assessment of physiological mechanisms using a comparative approach. J Comp Physiol B 180:785–796

National Institute of Population Studies (NIPS) [Pakistan] and ICF International (2013) Pakistan demographic and health survey 2012–13. Islamabad, Pakistan, and Calverton, Maryland, USA, NIPS and ICF International. https://dhsprogram.com/what-we-do/survey/survey-display-419.cfm . Accessed 25 Aug 2017

Pakistan Bureau of Statistics (PBS). Pakistan Social and Living Standards Measurements Survey, 2006–07, 2008–09, 2010–11, 2012–13. Government of Pakistan http://www.pbs.gov.pk/content/microdata . Accessed 25 Aug 2017

Quintana-Domeque C, Pedro Ródenas-Serrano P (2017) The hidden costs of terrorism: the effects on health at birth. J Health Econ 56:47–60

Sen A (1990) More than 100 million women are missing. The New York review of books. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Torche F (2011) The effect of maternal stress on birth outcomes: exploiting a natural experiment. Demography 48(4):1473–1491

Torche F, Kleinhaus K (2011) Prenatal stress, gestational age and secondary sex ratio: the sex-specific effects of exposure to a natural disaster in early pregnancy. Hum Reprod 27(2):558–567

Valente C (2015) Civil conflict, gender-specific fetal loss, and selection: a new test of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis. J Health Econ 39:31–50

Younas J (2015) Does globalization mitigate the adverse effects of terrorism on growth? Oxf Econ Pap 67(1):133–156

Younas J, Sandler T (2017) Gender imbalance and terrorism in developing countries. J Conflict Resolut 61(3):483–510

Zhang J (2017) The evolution of China’s one-child policy and its effects on family outcomes. J Econ Perspect 31(1):141–160

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Economics, American University of Sharjah, Sharjah, UAE

Khusrav Gaibulloev & Javed Younas

Dallas, TX, USA

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Javed Younas .

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Gaibulloev, K., Oyun, G. & Younas, J. Impact of terrorism on child sex at birth: evidence from Pakistan. Empir Econ (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-024-02605-z

Download citation

Received : 06 August 2023

Accepted : 24 April 2024

Published : 14 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-024-02605-z

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Child sex at birth
  • Parental stress
  • Birth outcomes

JEL Classification

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government, Department of Justice.

Here's how you know

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock A locked padlock ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

NCJRS Virtual Library

Evaluating and assessing terrorism prevention programs: what research sponsored by the national institute of justice tells us.

This document analyzes lessons and insights gleaned from terrorism prevention programs, and discusses their implications for future assessment and evaluation processes.

This report presents several projects that provide insights into National Institute of Justice (NIJ)-sponsored evaluations of terrorism prevention programs implemented across different populations, focusing on the process of program evaluation as well as evaluation findings. Important lessons that emerged from the research underscore the importance of gaining community buy-in and assuring program relevance for participants before implementing the programs; program benefits can extend beyond meeting terrorism prevention goals and can be carried out within public health or community resilience initiatives; and evaluative efforts in general face challenges that limit their ability to assess the impact of a program, its adaptability to other places or communities, and the veracity of evaluation findings. The evaluation findings suggest that several activities should be considered when addressing challenges, including the implementation of formative evaluations or evaluability assessments, strategies to limit participant attrition and thus maintaining appropriate sample sizes, and using control or comparison groups as part of the assessment. The report also discusses important gaps and considerations that should be incorporated into future programmatic and evaluation decisions, and suggests that future programmatic, research, and policy-oriented activities may explore and test efforts aimed at incorporating terrorism prevention activities with a broader portfolio of public health and violence reduction-focused efforts.

Additional Details

810 Seventh Street NW , Washington , DC 20531 , United States

Related Topics

  • Open access
  • Published: 10 May 2024

Navigating an unpredictable environment: the moderating role of perceived environmental unpredictability in the effectiveness of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior

  • Dian Gu 1 , 2 &
  • Jiang Jiang 3  

BMC Psychology volume  12 , Article number:  261 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

135 Accesses

6 Altmetric

Metrics details

The global issue of ecological resource scarcity, worsened by climate change, necessitates effective methods to promote resource conservation. One commonly used approach is presenting ecological resource scarcity information. However, the effectiveness of this method remains uncertain, particularly in an unpredictable world. This research aims to examine the role of perceived environmental unpredictability in moderating the impact of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior (PEB).

We conducted three studies to test our hypothesis on moderation. Study 1 ( N  = 256) measured perceived general environmental unpredictability, perceived resource scarcity and daily PEB frequencies in a cross-sectional survey. Study 2 ( N  = 107) took it a step further by manipulating resource scarcity. Importantly, to increase ecological validity, Study 3 ( N  = 135) manipulated the information on both ecological resource scarcity and nature-related environmental unpredictability, and measured real water and paper consumption using a newly developed washing-hands paradigm.

In Study 1, we discovered that perceived resource scarcity positively predicted PEB, but only when individuals perceive the environment as less unpredictable (interaction effect: 95% CI  = [-0.09, -0.01], Δ R 2  = 0.018). Furthermore, by manipulating scarcity information, Study 2 revealed that only for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability presenting ecological resource scarcity information could decrease forest resource consumption intention (interaction effect: 95% CI  = [-0.025, -0.031], Δ R 2  = .04). Moreover, Study 3 found that the negative effect of water resource scarcity information on actual water and (interaction effect: 95%CI = [3.037, 22.097], η p 2  = .050) paper saving behaviors (interaction effect: 95%CI = [0.021, 0.275], η p 2  = .040), as well as hypothetical forest resource consumption (interaction effect: 95%CI = [-0.053, 0.849], η p 2  = .023) emerged only for people who receiving weaker environmental unpredictability information.

Across three studies, we provide evidence to support the moderation hypothesis that environmental unpredictability weakens the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on PEB, offering important theoretical and practical implications on the optimal use of resource scarcity to enhance PEB.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Ecological resource scarcity, such as water and energy, poses significant challenges in our current times. The reduction of renewable freshwater resources per capita by 55% from 1993 to 2014 emphasizes the urgency of addressing this issue [ 1 ]. According to the World Economic Forum (2019), water shortages remain a top concern for policymakers and business leaders worldwide. In response to resource scarcity, various entities, including governments, water utilities, and community-based organizations, have employed different strategies to promote resource conservation [ 2 ]. One of the most common approaches is to raise problem awareness by conveying information about resource scarcity [ 2 ]. For example, the fact that billions of people lack access to safe water is utilized in the World Water Day campaign in 2023 to encourage more people to take action. Additionally, the Hong Kong SAR Government’s “Let’s Save 10L Water 2.0” campaign emphasizes the importance of conserving water by highlighting the limited availability of this resource.

Despite these efforts, it is important to recognize the complexity and interconnectedness of the world we live in, which makes predicting future environmental conditions challenging. Unforeseen events such as pathogen prevalence, natural disasters, wars, and financial crises illustrate the dynamic nature of our environment. In such an unpredictable world, can simply providing information about ecological resource scarcity lead to a significant increase in pro-environmental behaviors?

In the current research, we aimed to explore whether ecological resource scarcity information could promote pro-environmental behaviors effectively in the unpredictable world. We argued that ecological resource scarcity information is not necessarily useful in promoting pro-environmental behaviors and proposed that environmental unpredictability is a vital factor weakening the effect of ecological resource scarcity on resource consumption.

Uncertain association between ecological resource scarcity information and pro-environmental behaviors

Based on the information-motivation-behavioral skills (IMB) model, individuals are more likely to change their behavior when they are informed about a problem, along with being motivated to act and have skills to act [ 3 ]. In the environmental protection domain, there is a general lack of problem awareness about ecological resource scarcity [ 4 , 5 ]. This lack of awareness hinders individuals from engaging in pro-environmental behaviors (PEB), which refers to the actions that enhance the quality of the environment, regardless of the intent behind them [ 6 ]. Resource conservation campaigns often focus on resource scarcity information to encourage PEB [ 7 ]. In some empirical studies, the resource scarcity information was found to be effective. For example, individuals living in regions that experience drought have a higher tendency to make behavioral changes to conserve water [ 8 , 9 ]. People who perceived stronger ecological resource scarcity reported higher resource-saving behavioral frequencies [ 10 ], and indicated a higher frequency of PEB [ 11 ]. And water scarcity information was linked to a significant decrease in water use [ 12 , 13 , 14 ].

However, we identified some conflicting evidence. Information about resource scarcity is often not sufficient to reduce resource consumption in intervention [ 15 ], and the effectiveness of awareness campaigns is unclear [ 16 ]. For example, presenting the information about water resource scarcity only was evaluated as ineffective to promote water-saving behaviors by lay people [ 10 ]. Energy scarcity information was not strong enough to affect attitudes, intentions, and behaviors toward electricity energy saving [ 17 ]. Moreover, resource scarcity information failed to modify resource consumption behaviors in experimental settings [ 2 , 18 ].

The uncertain relationship between resource scarcity and PEB can be understood through an evolutionary psychological approach. According to the life history theory, individuals may adopt various strategies for allocating resources [ 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 ]. Those who choose a slow life history strategy prioritize long-term benefits and future planning, which leads them to behave in an environmentally friendly manner for the sake of future generations. On the other hand, individuals adopting a fast life history strategy prioritize immediate gains over long-term consequences [ 24 ], resulting in less PEB.

This theory, combined with empirical evidence, suggest that the impact of resource scarcity on PEB may vary depending on the situation, implying that promoting pro-environmental actions may require considering factors beyond simply informing individuals about scarcity. If PEB is seen as an investment in the environment, people engaging in PEB expect long-term benefits from it. However, the environment does not always provide consistent long-term benefits, particularly in today’s unpredictable world. When the expected advantages of environmental protection become uncertain, individuals may prioritize immediate gains, exploit natural resources, and reduce their commitment to PEB. This study hence focuses on the situational factor related to the unpredictable environment, testing its importance in influencing individuals’ PEB under resource scarcity.

Moderating role of environmental unpredictability

Environmental unpredictability is defined as the level of spatial–temporal variation in environmental harshness [ 24 ]. Past empirical studies measured environmental unpredictability in diverse ways [ 25 ]. In the current research, we tried to capture both individual-related and nature-related environmental unpredictability in temporal or spatial dimensions. Individual-related environmental unpredictability is mostly indicated by residential changes, and changes in parental financial status for children [ 19 , 24 , 26 ]. It shows whether the structure of an environment, such as the social or economic environment in which one lives, changes over time. Nature-related environmental unpredictability focuses on the pattern of variation that makes environments unpredictable, such as unpredictability of weather and the unpredictability of natural disasters [ 25 ].

Based on the life history theory, the environment plays a crucial role in shaping individuals’ life history strategies [ 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 ]. In predictable environments individuals are more likely to adopt a slow life-history strategy, while highly unpredictable environments promote a fast life-history strategy [ 24 ]. Importantly, environmental unpredictability during childhood can influence short-sighted tendencies [ 27 , 28 , 29 , 30 ], and this effect can also be observed in adulthood [ 31 ]. In an unpredictable environment, individuals prioritize immediate desires over future needs because investing in long-term environmental protection may not yield future benefits. This has implications for PEB, as present efforts on environmental protection may not be effective in improving resource scarcity in the future when the environment is unpredictable.

There are two aspects that illustrate the expectation that PEB efforts may not pay off in unpredictable environments. Firstly, in an unpredictable environment, there is a flow of uncontrollable information, which makes it challenging for individuals to maintain strong beliefs that their actions can bring about positive outcomes, such as improving resource scarcity [ 32 ]. According to the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior, the impact of awareness of the problem on behavior is greater when individuals perceive a higher level of control over their actions [ 33 ]. Hence, environmental unpredictability not only reduces the perceived personal control but also creates a barrier between scarcity awareness and PEB.

Secondly, in unpredictable environments, individuals are more likely to fear free riders, which further hinders behavioral change towards environmental protection under resource scarcity. When deciding whether to take action to protect the environment, people consider whether others will cooperate. However, in unpredictable environments, the likelihood of others investing in PEB becomes uncertain as well, which induces a heightened fear of free riders. For instance, experimental games have shown that individuals behave less cooperatively and invest fewer public goods when the probability of benefiting from them is uncertain [ 34 ]. Moreover, studies have demonstrated that individuals are less likely to prioritize the interests of others over their own when environmental unpredictability is primed [ 31 , 35 ]. Due to the fear that others will not take action in an unpredictable environment, individual efforts to protect the environment may appear less effective in solving the issue of resource scarcity.

Taken together, stronger environmental unpredictability is associated with a fast life-history strategy characterized by low self-efficacy and high fear of free riders, which ultimately leads to less PEB performance in the face of resource scarcity. Both multilevel and individual-level studies have indicated that psychological traits similar to the fast life history strategy weaken the association between environmental problem awareness and actual PEB [ 10 , 36 ]. Besides, some indirect evidence revealed that resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability could lead to some psychological outcomes that go against promoting PEB. Specifically, poorer childhood and economic uncertainty jointly increase the present orientation and decrease the sense of control [ 37 , 38 ]. A strong present orientation and low sense of control discourage people from taking actions to save resources [ 39 ]. With the above in mind, the following moderation hypothesis was proposed:

Hypothesis: Environmental unpredictability will moderate the effect of ecological resource scarcity on PEB. Specifically, ecological resource scarcity information would play a less effective role in promoting PEB when environmental unpredictability is stronger.

Current research

In the current research, we conducted three studies to test our hypothesis on moderation. In Study 1, we examined whether perceived general environmental unpredictability would moderate the relationship between perceived resource scarcity and daily PEB frequencies. Study 2 took it a step further by manipulating resource scarcity to test whether the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on forest resource consumption intention would be weakened by individual-related environmental unpredictability, specifically the frequency of residential changes. Importantly, to increase ecological validity, Study 3 manipulated the information on both ecological resource scarcity and nature-related environmental unpredictability, and measured real water and paper consumption using a newly developed washing-hands paradigm.

To examine the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on the relationship between ecological resource scarcity and daily PEB frequency, we conducted a cross-sectional survey for Study 1. We hypothesised that ecological resource scarcity would predict higher frequencies of daily PEB for individuals who perceived the environment as predictable. However, we expected this positive association to diminish for individuals who perceived high levels of environmental unpredictability.

Participants

To ensure sufficient statistical power (80% power, α = .05) to detect a small-to-medium-sized effect for our moderation hypothesis, based on previous research in the same domain [ 10 ], we estimated that a sample size of 256 participants would be required using G*Power 3.1 [ 40 ]. Participants were recruited from a Chinese online survey platform ( www.wjx.cn ) and received monetary compensation for their participation. The survey platform utilized a voluntary opt-in panel, inviting users to complete the questionnaire. A total of 263 participants from China completed the survey. It is important to note that data collection was planned to conclude once 256 observations were collected within a three-week period.

The average age of the participants was 32.21 ± 7.11 years (ranging from 18 to 66 years), with 44.1% of them being male ( N  = 116). In terms of educational attainment, 1.9% held a middle-school degree or below, 1.9% had a high school degree, 8.7% held a junior college degree, 79.8% had a bachelor’s degree, and 7.6% had a master’s degree or higher. The average annual family income was 23.65 ± 21.04 ten thousand yuan.

Procedure and measures

To address the potential influence of priming participants’ perceived resource scarcity through items expressing the seriousness of resource scarcity [ 11 , 41 , 42 ], we carefully structured the data collection process. Firstly, we measured the dependent variable, PEB frequencies. Following this, participants completed the measure of perceived environmental unpredictability, and subsequently rated their perceived ecological resource scarcity. Additionally, to account for potential bias in self-reported PEB due to social desirability [ 43 ], we included a measurement of social desirability as a control variable. Finally, participants provided their demographic information, including age, gender, educational attainment, and annual personal income.

Perceived resource scarcity

The measurement of perceived ecological resource scarcity, consisting of 5 items, was adapted from a previous study conducted by Gu and her colleagues [ 10 ] (Cronbach’s α in the current study is 0.79). Participants were asked to indicate their level of agreement with statements such as “There are not enough resources for everyone in the place where I live” and “In the place where I live, I have already noticed some signs of resource scarcity.” Each item was rated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 7 ( strongly agree ). The mean score of the entire scale was computed. Higher scores on this scale indicated higher levels of perceived ecological resource scarcity.

Perceived general environmental unpredictability

The item “For me, the environment we live in is unpredictable” developed by Reynolds and McCrea [ 44 ], was used to measure how participants perceived the general unpredictability of their environment. Participants rated this item on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 7 ( strongly agree ). Higher score indicated stronger perceived unpredictability.

Daily PEB frequency

Participants were asked to rate the frequency of PEB in their daily lives on a scale from 1 ( never ) to 5 ( always ). They were presented with six common resource conservation actions and asked to consider their behaviors in the year prior to the survey. The items are “do not turn the tap to the maximum when using water”, “switch off the lights when you leave”, “set the air conditioner’s temperature to 26–28 degrees centigrade in summer”, “buy and use energy-efficient appliances”, “avoid using disposable tableware whenever possible”. These six PEB were then converted into a PEB frequency scale, and a mean score was calculated for each participant. Higher scores indicated a higher frequency of PEB. Although the Cronbach’s α for the PEB scale was relatively low at .50, we decided to keep the measure because the items were face-valid. It is worth noting that removing any of the items did not improve the Cronbach’s alpha. Consistent with findings from previous studies, different types of PEB were not completely consistent [ 45 , 46 ]. And importantly, using the common score derived from the six items did not significantly alter the results.

Social desirability

Social desirability was measured using the liar subscale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) [ 47 ]. This subscale consists of 12 items, with participants answering each question with a “ Yes ” or “ No ” response. A code of 1 was assigned to “ Yes ” and 0 to “ No ”. Higher scores on this subscale indicated a stronger tendency towards social desirability. The measure demonstrated good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α of .75.

Correlation analyses

Prior to conducting hypothesis testing, all variables exhibited normal distributions, as indicated by skewness values ranging from -0.89 to + 0.05 and kurtosis values ranging from -0.72 to + 0.77. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients to explore the relations among the studied variables (see Table  1 for descriptive statistics and intercorrelation coefficients). We found a marginally significant positive relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB frequency ( r  = 0.12, p  = .058). And there was no correlation between environmental unpredictability and PEB frequency ( r  = -0.07, p  = .29). Importantly, as expected, social desirability was positively associated with PEB ( r  = 0.30, p  < .001), indicating that it should be controlled for in subsequent analyses.

Moderation analyses

To examine the impact of environmental unpredictability on the relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB, we used the PROCESS macro for SPSS [ 48 ]. Controlling for social desirability, we found a significant interaction effect between perceived ecological resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability ( b  = -0.05, SE  = 0.02, t  = -2.26, p  = .025, 95% CI  = [-0.09, -0.01], Δ R 2  = 0.018). To further understand this interaction, we conducted a floodlight analysis [ 49 ]. The results showed that perceived ecological resource scarcity was positively and significantly associated with PEB when environmental unpredictability was below 4.41 ( b  = 0.07, SE  = 0.03, t  = 1.97, p  = .05, 95% CI  = [0.000, 0.136]), but not when it was above 4.41.

Additionally, we performed a simple slope analysis to examine the relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB for individuals with different levels of perceived environmental unpredictability with social desirability controlled (see Fig.  1 ). The results indicated that perceived ecological resource scarcity positively predicted PEB for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability (-1 SD ), b  = 0.13, SE  = 0.05, t  = 2.83, p  = .005, 95% CI  = [0.039, 0.219]. However, this relationship was not significant for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability (+ 1 SD ), ( b  = -0.02, SE  = 0.05, t  = -0.35, p  = 0.73, 95% CI  = [-0.114, 0.079]).

figure 1

The effect of resource scarcity on PEB at different levels of environmental unpredictability (Study 1)

Furthermore, controlling for demographic variables did not significantly change the results of moderation analysis. In summary, individuals who perceived the environment as more predictable were more likely to engage in PEB when facing ecological resource scarcity.

Brief discussion

Study 1 identified a moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on associations between perceived ecological resource scarcity and daily PEB. Individuals who perceived the environment as less unpredictable were more likely to adopt environmentally friendly ways to respond to ecological resource scarcity. However, it is important to consider the potential influence of responding to the PEB items on participants’ perceptions of ecological resource scarcity. The act of responding to these items may have directed participants’ attention towards environmental issues, potentially leading to an implicit increase in their perceived ecological resource scarcity. Therefore, it is not possible to infer the direction of the causal relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB frequencies solely from correlational data. In addition, using a single item for measuring environmental unpredictability may raise concerns about the comprehensiveness of measurement. To address these limitations, we conducted Study 2, where we manipulated perceived ecological resource scarcity in order to demonstrate its causal effect, and further explore the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability by using another measurement.

Furthermore, it is important to note that the observed moderation effect size was small, which could be attributed to the fact that we measured various types of PEB in this study. According to the Goal System Theory, PEB can be motivated by multiple goals. In the context of resource scarcity, individuals who perceive the environment as more predictable are more likely to prioritize environmental protection for the benefit of future generations, especially if they themselves also stand to gain [ 50 ]. For instance, engaging in electricity-saving behaviors not only benefits the environment in the long run but also reduces personal electricity bills. In other words, personal benefits may matter. In our subsequent studies, we will focus on examining PEB that does not involve salient personal benefits in order to highlight the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability.

In Study 2, we sought to replicate the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on the link between ecological resource scarcity and PEB by manipulating resource scarcity information. We proposed that receiving ecological resource scarcity information would increase PEB intention for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability but that the effect would disappear for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability.

To test our moderation hypothesis, we determined that a sample size of 107 would be necessary to achieve 80% power (α = .05) in order to detect a small-to-medium-size effect ( f 2  = .075) based on previous research [ 10 ] using G*Power 3.1 [ 40 ]. We established the rule for ending data collection prior to gathering data, stipulating that the survey link would be closed after obtaining more than 150 observations. Ultimately, we recruited 155 Chinese adults who completed an anonymous online questionnaire and all of these responses were valid.

The participants had an average age of 32.91 ± 10.10 years (range = 18–59 years) and 41.90% of them were males ( N  = 65). In terms of educational attainment, 9.70% held a high school degree, 16.1% held a junior college degree, 66.6% held a bachelor’s degree, and 13.5% held a master’s degree or higher. The average annual personal income was 11.18 ± 44.58 ten thousand yuan.

In the present study, participants reported their demographic information first. Then, environmental unpredictability was measured. Next, participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions to read a news article, where exposure to the information of resource scarcity (vs. control condition) was the manipulated factor. Finally, PEB intention was measured using a forest management task.

Manipulation of ecological resource scarcity information

Participants were assigned at random to read one of two news articles. The articles were created specifically to manipulate perceptions of ecological resource scarcity. In the scarcity group ( n  = 77), participants read an article titled “Interpretation of China’s Resources through Big Data: Invisible Resource Scarcity in China”, which highlighted the severity of natural resource scarcity in China. In the control group ( n  = 78), participants read an article of similar length that aimed to evoke similar levels of negative arousal. This article was titled “Interpretation of Sleep through Big Data: Invisible Sleeping Problems in China” and discussed sleep issues in China. To ensure the credibility of the mock news articles, participants were informed that the articles were sourced from The People’s Daily , a reputable Chinese newspaper.

Immediately after reading their respective article, participants rated their perception of ecological resource scarcity using a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “ strongly disagree ” (1) to “ strongly agree ” (7). The item presented was: “Currently, I believe that we live in an environment where natural resources are extremely scarce.” Besides, participants also responded to one item on their mood at the moment for the manipulation check on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “ very negative ” to 7 = “ very positive ”).

  • Environmental unpredictability

At the individual level, environmental unpredictability is mostly indicated by residential changes [ 24 , 25 ]. The frequency of residential changes showed whether the structure of an environment one lives in changes over time, which is the important aspect of environmental unpredictability. Therefore, Study 2 used the frequency individuals moved in the past to represent their environmental unpredictability. Higher score indicates stronger environmental unpredictability ( M  = 3.59, SD  = 2.17, Min  = 0, Max  = 11). The variable showed approximately normal distribution, with skewness = 0.64 and kurtosis = 0.65. Hence, the raw data of moving frequency are used for analysis.

PEB intention

A forest management task was used to measure PEB intention, specifically in relation to forest resource conservation intention [ 51 ]. Participants were asked to imagine that they were the owner of a timber company and must compete with three other companies to harvest timber in the same forest. They need to cut down as many trees as possible for their companies to profit and thrive. However, the rapid deforestation could lead to forest destruction. Then, participants were asked to answer one question about deforestation rate on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 ( very slow ) to 7 ( very fast ), which asked, ‘How fast do you want your company to cut down trees?’ Additionally, they were asked one question about forest resource consumption, ranging from 1 to 100 acres, which asked, ‘How many acres of trees do you expect your company to cut down?’. Give that both questions indicate greedy for forest resources, the average of participants’ reversed standardized scores on the two questions was computed to represent PEB intention. Higher scores indicate stronger forest resource conservation intention. We also treated the two items separately to test our hypothesis, which can be found in the Additional file 1 .

Manipulation checks

The manipulation of resource scarcity information was successful. Specifically, participants in the scarcity condition ( M  = 5.17, SD  = 1.25) compared to those in the control condition ( M  = 4.55, SD  = 1.56), reported higher levels of awareness on ecological resource scarcity, t (153) = 2.72, p  = .007, 95%CI = [0.169, 1.066], d  = 0.44. Furthermore, there was no difference of mood between the two conditions ( M scarcity  = 5.06, SD scarcity  = 1.19; M control  = 4.92, SD control  = 1.23), t (153) = 0.73, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.526, 0.242].

Hypothesis test

To test for the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability, we regressed the forest resource conservation intention on ecological resource scarcity information (dummy coded: 1 =  scarcity condition, 0 =  control condition), environmental unpredictability and their interaction by employing the PROCESS macro (Model 1, 5000 bootstrap samples) for SPSS [ 48 ]. The results showed a significant main effect of ecological resource scarcity information ( b  = 0.63, SE  = 0.23, t  = 2.78, p  = .006, 95% CI  = [0.183, 1.078]). And there was no main effect of environmental unpredictability ( b  = 0.04, SE  = .03, t  = 1.23, p  > .05, 95% CI  = [-0.026, 0.109]).

Results showed a significant interaction effect ( b  = -0.14, SE  = 0.06, t  = -2.54, p  = .012, 95% CI  = [-0.025, -0.031], Δ R 2  = .04), meaning that the effect of ecological resource scarcity information on forest resource conservation intention was moderated by environmental unpredictability. Specifically, for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability (below 1 SD ), participants in the scarcity condition exhibited stronger forest resource conservation intention relative to those in the control condition, b  = 0.43, SE  = 0.16, t  = 2.63, p  = .0095, 95% CI = [0.107, 0.755]. In contrast, for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability (above 1 SD ), the ecological resource scarcity manipulation had no effect on forest resource conservation intention, b  = -0.17, SE  = 0.17, t  = -1.04, p  > .05, 95% CI = [-0.512, 0.158] (see Fig.  2 ).

figure 2

The effect of resource scarcity × environmental unpredictability on forest resource conservation intention (Study 2)

Besides, a floodlight analysis was performed to decompose the interaction [ 49 ]. It revealed that ecological resource scarcity manipulation increased forest resource conservation intention for any value of environmental unpredictability less than 2.78 ( b  = 0.24, SE  = 0.12, t  = 1.98, p  = .05, 95% CI = [0.000, 0.487]), but not for any value greater than 2.78. More importantly, the above findings did not significantly differ after controlling for demographic variables.

Study 2 replicated results of Study 1 and identified that environmental unpredictability weakened the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on resource conservation. Presenting ecological resource scarcity information could effectively increase forest conservation intention, particularly for individuals who move less frequently, indicating lower levels of environmental unpredictability.

However, the results of Study 2 were limited in several aspects. First, environmental unpredictability can be caused either by individuals themselves, such as frequent relocation, or by nature, such as unforeseen natural disasters. The present study focused on individual-related environmental unpredictability only. Secondly, the measurement of resource conservation intention instead of actual behaviors may have restricted the ecological validity of the findings. Thirdly, it is possible that the moderation effect was underestimated. In the forest management task, the psychological experience of forest resource scarcity may have been primed in both conditions, as participants were informed about the need to compete with other companies for limited forest resources. Consequently, participants’ decisions may have been heavily influenced by the forest management scenario.

Based on above discussions of Study 2, in Study 3, actual PEB was measured to increase ecological validity, and nature-caused environmental unpredictability was focused to improve generalizability. In addition, hypothetical forest resource conservation was also measured to replicate findings of Study 2. We proposed that receiving ecological resource scarcity information would increase actual resource conservation and forest resource conservation intention under predictable environmental conditions but that this effect would disappear under unpredictable environmental conditions.

We conducted a power analysis through G*Power 3.1 with the moderating effect size in Study 2, which suggested that a sample size of 135 would be required to achieve 80% power ( α  = .05) [ 40 ]. A total of 142 college students in Beijing, China was recruited to participate in the experiment in exchange for monetary compensation. Six participants who failed to finish all experimental tasks were excluded from data analysis. It is worth noting that the rule for terminating data collection was decided before data collection began: the experiment was terminated when more than 135 observations were collected in two weeks.

The average age of the participants was 21.87 ± 2.67 years (range = 17–29 years), and 75.00% of them were female ( N  = 102). The average annual household income was 12.37 ± 17.10 thousand yuan .

Research design and procedure

A 2 (water resource scarcity vs. control) × 2 (unpredictable vs. predictable environment) between-subject design was used.

Before arriving at the lab, participants were asked to fill out their demographic information in an online survey. Upon arrival at the lab, participants were randomly assigned into one of four groups to read a newspaper. These newspapers were designed to be looked like real Beijing Daily newspapers. In each type of newspaper, there were two pieces of news. One was designed to manipulate the water resource scarcity information, and another was designed to manipulate environmental unpredictability information. Then, actual water and paper consumption data was recorded in a washing-hands paradigm. Finally, forest resource consumption intention was measured.

Manipulation of water resource scarcity information

Similar to Study 2, in the scarcity condition ( n  = 67), the news article described the seriousness of water resource scarcity in Beijing. While, in the control condition ( n  = 69), the news article described Beijing residents’ sleep problems. After reading the article, participants responded to 1 item on perceived ecological resource scarcity on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “ strongly disagree ” to 7 = “ strongly agree ”), which was adapted from new ecological paradigm scale (NEP): “The earth has plenty of natural resources if we just learn how to develop them” [ 52 ].

Manipulation of environmental unpredictability information

In the unpredictable condition ( n  = 68), the news article was titled “Natural Disasters are Unpredictable and Difficult to Prevent: 9.578 million People were Affected by Various Natural Disasters in January”. The news conveyed the information that natural disasters happened frequently, which caused many people to be affected in January, and there was no way to predict and prevent disasters. By contrast, in the predictable condition ( n  = 68), the news stated that even though natural disasters are frequent in China and many people were affected, now some devices can help predict and prevent disasters. The title was “Predication and Prevention of the Occurrence of Natural Disasters is Possible: 9.578 million People were Affected by Various Natural Disasters in January”.

Manipulation check items were rated right after reading the news article. Participants responded to 2 items about perceived unpredictability on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “ strongly disagree ” to 7 = “ strongly agree ”): “The environment where I live is unstable”, and “The environment where I live is unpredictable”. The average score of the two items was computed such that a higher score indicated stronger perceived unpredictability.

Actual water and paper resource consumption

To cover up our real purpose, participants were told that the research was attempting to study palms, so that we would collect their fingerprints in the study. In the washing-hands paradigm, participants were asked to use the inkpad and leave their fingerprints on a sheet of white paper to study their palms. After that, they had to wash their hands in the lab. The amount of water and paper they used was recorded.

To measure the water consumption, the experimenter placed one measuring cylinder under the washbasin, and the measuring cylinder was linked to the washbasin’s outlet pipe. Importantly, participants could not see the cylinder. To measure their paper consumption, a bag of paper was placed on the washbasin for the participants to use. Besides, to exclude the experimenter effect, participants washed their hands without experimenter observation. Importantly, participants did not know that their behaviors were recorded, and participants were not aware of the real purpose of the study (see Fig.  3 ). All of the participants were debriefed at the end of the study.

figure 3

Set-up of washing-hands paradigm

Considering that water and paper consumption for washing ink from hands might be affected by palm size, we recorded the palm area for each participant based on their fingerprints. Then, actual resource consumption was represented by average water consumption and average paper consumption, calculated by water or paper consumption divided by palm area.

Hypothetical forest resource conservation

Same as Study 2, the forest management task was used. After reading the scenario, participants were asked to answer the question, “How many acres of trees do you expect your company to cut down?”, ranging from 1 to 100 acres. A higher score on the measurement indicates a lower intention for forest resource conservation.

Perceived resource scarcity was significantly greater in the scarcity condition ( n  = 67, M  = 5.61, SD  = 1.19) than that in the control condition ( n  = 69, M  = 5.13, SD  = 1.38), t (134) = 2.17, p  = .032, 95%CI = [0.043, 0.920], d  = 0.37. Perceived unpredictability was also significantly greater in the unpredictable condition ( n  = 68, M  = 5.13, SD  = 1.28) compared to the predictable condition ( n  = 68, M  = 4.55, SD  = 1.39), t (134) = 2.51, p  = .013, 95%CI = [0.121, 1.026], d  = 0.43. Overall, the manipulations were successful and valid.

To examine the interaction effect between water resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability on resource conservation, two-factor MANOVAs were conducted.

Concerning the average water consumption, gender, age, household income, and cleanliness habits are included as control variables. The findings revealed that the main effect of scarcity was significant ( F (1,128) = 5.44, p  = 0.021, 95%CI = [-14.168, -0.673], η p 2  = .041), and the main effect of environmental unpredictability was not significant ( F (1,128) = 0.23, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-7.437, 5.984]). As expected, the interaction was significant ( F (1,128) = 6.81, p  = .01, 95%CI = [3.037, 22.097], η p 2  = .050). Then, simple effect analysis revealed that under the predictable condition, the average water consumption was significantly less under the scarcity condition ( M  = 25.29, SD  = 13.87) than under the control condition ( M  = 37.13, SD  = 13.91), F (1,128) = 12.25, p  < .001, 95%CI = [-18.535, -5.146], η p 2  = .087. However, under the unpredictable condition, there was no significant difference between the scarcity condition ( M  = 32.71, SD  = 13.93) and control condition ( M  = 31.98, SD  = 13.90), F (1,128) = 0.05, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-5.984, 7.437] (see Fig.  4 ).

figure 4

Average water consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

Moreover, the results in average paper consumption showed a similar pattern. Main effects of scarcity ( F (1,128) = 0.42, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.137, 0.042]) and environmental unpredictability ( F (1,128) = 0.70, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.143, 0.036]) were not significant. A significant interaction effect was detected, F (1,128) = 5.30, p  = .023, 95%CI = [0.021, 0.275], η p 2  = .040. As predicted, in the predictable condition, paper consumption in the scarcity condition ( M  = 0.28, SD  = 0.18) was significantly less than in the control condition ( M  = 0.38, SD  = 0.18), F (1,128) = 4.39, p  = .038, 95%CI = [-0.184, -0.005], η p 2  = .033. No significant difference in paper consumption were observed between scarcity condition ( M  = 0.33, SD  = 0.19) and control condition ( M  = 0.28, SD  = 0.19) in unpredictable condition, F (1,128) = 1.39, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.036, 0.143] (see Fig.  5 ).

figure 5

Average paper consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

More importantly, the above findings did not significantly differ without control variables in data analysis, and also did not significantly differ using the raw scores of water and paper consumption. Detailed results can be found in the Additional file 1 .

Hypothetical forest resource consumption was log transformed as it showed non-normal distribution. The findings showed that the main effects of scarcity ( F (1,130) = 1.800, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.363, 0.271]) and environmental unpredictability ( F (1,130) = 2.189, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.688, 0.049]) were not significant. A marginally significant interaction effect was detected, F (1, 130) = 3.04, p  = .084, 95%CI = [-0.053, 0.849], η p 2  = .023. As predicted, in the predictable condition, forest resource consumption in the scarcity condition ( M raw  = 30.76, SD raw  = 14.97) was significantly less than the control condition ( M raw  = 40.74, SD raw  = 25.41), F (1,130) = 4.71, p  = .032, 95%CI = [-0.672, -0.031], η p 2  = .035. No significant difference of forest resource consumption was observed between scarcity condition ( M raw  = 42.15, SD raw  = 20.41) and control condition ( M raw  = 44.00, SD raw  = 28.17) in unpredictable condition, F (1,130) = 0.08, p  > .05, 95%CI = [-0.027, 0.363] (see Fig.  6 ).

figure 6

Forest resource consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

As expected, Study 3 replicated the findings of the previous two studies. We identified a moderating effect of nature-caused environmental unpredictability on ecological resource scarcity information’s effect on actual PEB. Specifically, individuals who received lower levels of environmental unpredictability information exhibited more water-saving and paper-saving behaviors, and were inclined to harvest fewer forest resources in the face of water scarcity. Interestingly, even though our manipulation focused solely on water scarcity, both paper consumption and forest resource consumption were affected as well, despite their lack of direct association with water. These results highlight the robust influence of resource scarcity information and environmental unpredictability on PEB, thereby enhancing the ecological validity of our findings.

General discussion

Focusing on the global issue of environmental unpredictability, the current research explored when does showing resource scarcity information promote PEB. In Study 1, a cross-sectional study, we discovered that resource scarcity information effectively enhances PEB, but only when individuals perceive the environment as less unpredictable. Furthermore, by manipulating scarcity information, Study 2 revealed that only for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability could presenting ecological resource scarcity information decrease forest resource consumption intention. Moreover, an experiment with high ecological validity was conducted in Study 3 and found that the negative effect of water resource scarcity information on actual water and paper saving behaviors, as well as hypothetical forest resource consumption emerged only for people who receiving weaker environmental unpredictability information.

Theoretical contribution and practical implication

Environmental unpredictability is an important concept in life history theory. Numerous studies have verified that childhood environmental unpredictability plays a crucial role in shaping life history strategies [ 27 , 28 , 30 , 37 , 53 ]. However, little is known about how adulthood environmental unpredictability functions. The current research provided preliminary evidence that unpredictability in adulthood can also function in shaping behaviors. Adulthood unpredictability, including both individual- and nature-related environmental unpredictability, demotivates individuals to sacrifice present interests for future environmental benefits when facing scarcity.

Some psychological factors, including those discussed earlier (such as short-sighted tendency, fear of free riders, and perceived lack of control), as well as self-interest and competitive orientation, can serve as potential mechanisms underlying the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability. Self-interest and competitive orientation are important ways for individuals to survive in a harsh environment. Individuals may adopt a competitive orientation to obtain more benefits for themselves to survive during periods of scarcity. In addition, they may also seek to weaken others’ interests. These factors have been identified as “Stone Age” psychological biases leading to environment destruction [ 54 ]. To better respond to ecological resource scarcity, the current research demonstrated the importance of creating a predictable and peaceful world by removing the psychological barriers to mitigate ecological resource scarcity.

The IMB model provides a comprehensive framework advancing resource conservation research and intervention implements [ 3 ]. Even though the IMB model captures three vital components, information, motivation, and behavioral skills on behavior change, the psychological barriers caused by environmental unpredictability were ignored. As illustrated in a recent meta-analysis [ 15 ], compared with the control group, of the 38 interventions including IMB components, water use was reduced by only 5.9% in average with a small effect size, and the magnitude of effect varied widely in different interventions. According to the findings in the current research, levels of environmental unpredictability may be the underlying reason for the varied efficacy. Therefore, to best strengthen reducing resource consumption interventions based on the IMB model, it’s necessary to take environmental unpredictability into consideration.

Importantly, the current research developed a new paradigm, washing-hands paradigm, to measure actual resource consumption in the lab. As illustrated in previous studies, there are gaps between self-reported behaviors, and objective behaviors [ 43 ]. However, over 80% of recent studies only relied on self-reported data [ 55 ]. The washing-hands paradigm sets up a situation to capture actual water and paper resource consumption data. Importantly, the confounding variables can be controlled in the paradigm, such as habits, individual difference on palm size, and social desirability. This paradigm can help to establish causality and improve ecological validity of lab experiments, advancing resource conservation research.

The current research also provides some vital practical implications for both policy makers and environmental organizations. Our data suggested that creating a predictable environment can help promote resource conservation when facing ecological resource scarcity information. Governments should try to eliminate unpredictable factors. However, some unpredictable factors are difficult to address, such as natural disasters and virus spread. In such conditions, individual-level practices appear to be more important. For countries with a predictable environment, the strategy of the reminders of the ecological resource scarcity information is effective. However, for countries with an unpredictable environment, governments and organizations can consider using public media to decrease residents’ perceived unpredictability. Moreover, inspired by our Study 2, emphasizing predictable environmental information when reminding residents of scarcity should be encouraged. Environmental organizations should provide information that the environment is predictable when calling for resource conservation to respond to scarcity.

Limitations and future directions

The current research faces the limitation that the measurement in the correlation study is restricted due to the use of only one item to measure the moderator, and the alpha level of the PEB measure is low. For future studies, one aspect to consider is the exploration potential mechanisms of the moderation hypothesis. The current research did not delve into psychological mechanisms. It is suggested that future research could investigate underlying potential mechanisms of the moderation hypothesis to enrich the framework. Another related issue pertains to the IMB model. In the current research, we mainly focused on the effectiveness of scarcity information component but didn’t include motivation and behavioral skills components. It’s worthy for future research to test if creating a predictable environment can still strengthen the effect of IMB intervention. Besides, there are various types of resource conservation behaviors that individuals can engage in. Importantly, different behaviors are not necessarily highly relevant. For example, factors predicting shutting down electronics at night could not predict upgrading to energy-efficient appliances because these behaviors may cluster into distinct dimensions [ 56 , 57 ]. In the current research, we may not be able to generalize our findings to other types of behaviors. Thus, future research is encouraged to investigate whether the moderation hypothesis can be verified in other types of resource conservation behaviors.

Across three studies, we provided evidence to support the moderation hypothesis that environmental unpredictability weakens the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on PEB. Moving forward, it would be valuable to delve deeper into the underlying mechanisms, examine the moderation effect across various types of PEB, and investigate its potential application in PEB interventions.

Availability of data and materials

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

World Bank. World development indicators 2016. Washington, DC: World Bank; 2016.

Book   Google Scholar  

Syme GJ, Nancarrow BE, Seligman C. The evaluation of information campaigns to promote voluntary household water conservation. Eval Rev. 2000;24(6):539–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193841x0002400601 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Fisher WA, Fisher JD, Shuper PA. Social psychology and the fight against AIDS: an information-motivation-behavioral skills model for the prediction and promotion of health behavior. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2014;50:105–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800284-1.00003-5 .

Famiglietti JS. The global groundwater crisis. Nat Clim Chang. 2014;4(11):945–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2425 .

Fisher JD, Fisher WA, Amico KR, Harman J. An information-motivation- behavioral skills model of adherence to antiretroviral therapy. Health Psychol. 2006;25(4):462–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.4.462 .

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Steg L, Perlaviciute G, van der Werff E, Lurvink J. The significance of hedonic values for environmentally relevant attitudes, preferences, and actions. Environ Behav. 2014;46(2):163–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512454730 .

Ehret PJ, Hodges HE, Kuehl C, Brick C, Mueller S, Anderson SE. Systematic review of household water conservation interventions using the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. Environ Behav. 2020;53(5):001391651989686. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916519896868 .

Hannibal B, Sansom L, E. Portney K. The effect of local water scarcity and drought on water conservation behaviors. Environ Sociol. 2019;5(3):294–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/23251042.2018.1519882 .

Rodriguez-Sanchez C, Sarabia-Sanchez FJ. Does water context matter in water conservation decision behaviour? Sustainability. 2020;12(7):3026. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12073026 .

Gu D, Jiang J, Zhang Y, Sun Y, Jiang W, Du X. Concern for the future and saving the earth: when does ecological resource scarcity promote pro-environmental behavior? J Environ Psychol. 2020;72(101501):101501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101501 .

Berthold A, Cologna V, Siegrist M. The influence of scarcity perception on people’s pro-environmental behavior and their readiness to accept new sustainable technologies. Ecol Econ. 2022;196(107399):107399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2022.107399 .

Katz D, Grinstein A, Kronrod A, Nisan U. Evaluating the effectiveness of a water conservation campaign: combining experimental and field methods. J Environ Manage. 2016;180:335–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2016.05.049 .

March H, Domènech L, Saurí D. Water conservation campaigns and citizen perceptions: the drought of 2007–2008 in the Metropolitan Area of Barcelona. Nat Hazards (Dordr). 2013;65(3):1951–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-012-0456-2 .

Quesnel KJ, Ajami NK. Changes in water consumption linked to heavy news media coverage of extreme climatic events. Sci Adv. 2017;3(10):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1700784 .

Ehret PJ, Hodges HE, Kuehl C, Brick C, Mueller S, Anderson SE. Systematic review of household water conservation interventions using the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. Environ Behav. 2021;53(5):485–519. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916519896868 .

Inman D, Jeffrey P. A review of residential water conservation tool performance and influences on implementation effectiveness. Urban Water J. 2006;3(3):127–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/15730620600961288 .

Fatmawati I, Dharmmesta BS, Purwanto BM, Nugroho SS. Promoting young adults to perform energy saving behavior through message framing: a lesson learned from Indonesia. Acad Strateg Manag J. 2018;17(5):1–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011 .

Kimbrough EO, Vostroknutov A. The social and ecological determinants of common pool resource sustainability. J Environ Econ Manag. 2015;72(C):38–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeem.2015.04.004l .

Belsky J, Schlomer GL, Ellis BJ. Beyond cumulative risk: distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):662–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454 .

Charnov EL. Life history invariants: some explorations of symmetry in evolutionary ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993.

Roff DA, ed. Evolution of life histories: theory and analysis. 1993rd ed. NY: Chapman and Hall; 1993.

Roff DA. Life history evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002.

Stearns SC. The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1992.

Ellis BJ, Figueredo AJ, Brumbach BH, Schlomer GL. Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: the impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Hum Nat. 2009;20(2):204–68. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7 .

Young ES, Frankenhuis WE, Ellis BJ. Theory and measurement of environmental unpredictability. Evol Hum Behav. 2020;41(6):550–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.08.006 .

Simpson JA, Griskevicius V, Kuo SIC, Sung S, Collins WA. Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: the influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):674–86. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027293 .

Chen BB, Qu W. Life history strategies and procrastination: the role of environmental unpredictability. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;117:23–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.036 .

Chen BB, Shi Z, Sun S. Life history strategy as a mediator between childhood environmental unpredictability and adulthood personality. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;111:215–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.032 .

Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Nettle D. Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016;7:76–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011 .

Hill EM, Jenkins J, Farmer L. Family unpredictability, future discounting, and risk taking. J Socio Econ. 2008;37(4):1381–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2006.12.081 .

Zhang Y, Gao Y, Jiang J. An unpredictable environment reduces pro-environmental behavior: a dynamic public goods experiment on forest use. J Environ Psychol. 2021;78(101702):101702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101702 .

Brumbach BH, Figueredo AJ, Ellis BJ. Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Hum Nat. 2009;20(1):25–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9059-3 .

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Ajzen I, Fishbein M. The influence of attitudes on behavior. In: Albarracín D, ed. The handbook of attitudes, Vol 826. Mahwah: Psychology Press; 2005. p. 173–221.

Wit A, Wilke H. Public good provision under environmental and social uncertainty. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1998;28(2):249–56. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199803/04)28:2%3C249::AID-EJSP868%3E3.0.CO;2-J .

Zhu N, Hawk ST, Chang L. Unpredictable and competitive cues affect prosocial behaviors and judgments. Pers Individ Dif. 2019;138:203–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.006 .

Tam KP, Chan HW. Generalized trust narrows the gap between environmental concern and pro-environmental behavior: Multilevel evidence. Glob Environ Change. 2018;48:182–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.12.001 .

Griskevicius V, Ackerman JM, Cantú SM, et al. When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(2):197–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612451471 .

Mittal C, Griskevicius V. Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people’s childhood environment: a life history theory approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2014;107(4):621–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037398 .

Gifford R. The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. Am Psychol. 2011;66(4):290–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023566 .

Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 .

Lee AJ, Zietsch BP. Experimental evidence that women’s mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity. Biol Lett. 2011;7(6):892–5. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.0454 .

Watkins CD, DeBruine LM, Little AC, Feinberg DR, Jones BC. Priming concerns about pathogen threat versus resource scarcity: dissociable effects on women’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness and dominance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012;66(12):1549–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-012-1408-2 .

Kormos C, Gifford R. The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: a meta-analytic review. J Environ Psychol. 2014;40:359–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.09.003 .

Reynolds JJ, McCrea SM. Life history theory and exploitative strategies. Evol Psychol. 2016;14(3):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916659483 .

Barr S. Factors influencing environmental attitudes and behaviors: a U.k. case study of household waste management. Environ Behav. 2007;39(4):435–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916505283421 .

Bratt C, Stern PC, Matthies E, Nenseth V. Home, car use, and vacation: the structure of environmentally significant individual behavior. Environ Behav. 2015;47(4):436–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/001391651452503 .

Eysenck H, Eysenck SB. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire : (Epq-R Adult). CA: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Service; 1994.

Hancock GR, Mueller RO, eds. Structural equation modeling: a second course. 2nd ed. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing; 2013.

Spiller SA, Fitzsimons GJ, Lynch JG Jr, Mcclelland GH. Spotlights, floodlights, and the magic number zero: simple effects tests in moderated regression. J Mark Res. 2013;50(2):277–88. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.12.0420 .

Kruglanski AW, Shah JY, Fishbach A, Friedman R, Chun WY, Sleeth-Keppler D. A theory of goal systems. In: Advances in experimental social psychology. NY: Elsevier; 2002. p. 331–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80008-9 .

Wang L, Gu D, Jiang J, Sun Y. The not-so-dark side of materialism: can public versus private contexts make materialists less Eco-unfriendly? Front Psychol. 2019;10:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00790 .

Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE. New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. J Soc Issues. 2000;56(3):425–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00176 .

White AE, Li YJ, Griskevicius V, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT. Putting all your eggs in one basket: life-history strategies, bet hedging, and diversification. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(5):715–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612461919 .

van Vugt M, Griskevicius V, Schultz PW. Naturally green: harnessing stone age psychological biases to foster environmental behavior. Soc Issues Policy Rev. 2014;8(1):1–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12000 .

Lange F, Steinke A, Dewitte S. The pro-environmental behavior task: a laboratory measure of actual pro-environmental behavior. J Environ Psychol. 2018;56:46–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.007 .

Karlin B, Davis N, Sanguinetti A, Gamble K, Kirkby D, Stokols D. Dimensions of conservation: exploring differences among energy behaviors. Environ Behav. 2014;46(4):423–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512467532 .

Nair G, Gustavsson L, Mahapatra K. Factors influencing energy efficiency investments in existing Swedish residential buildings. Energy Policy. 2010;38(6):2956–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2010.01.033 .

Download references

The authors acknowledge the financial support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31871126), Chongqing Normal University (23XWB043) and Social Science Fund of Chongqing, China (2023BS076).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Key Laboratory of Applied Psychology, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China

School of Education, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China

Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education (Beijing Normal University), Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, No.19 Xinjiekouwai Street, Beijing, 100875, China

Jiang Jiang

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

GD and JJ contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by GD. GD and JJ contributed to the interpretation of data. The first draft of the manuscript was written by GD. JJ commented on previous versions of the manuscript or revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jiang Jiang .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The study was conducted after getting approval from the Institutional Review Board of the Beijing Normal University and was conducted following the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations of Beijing Normal University. Participants were informed that their names and institution names would be kept confidential and their privacy rights were protected. Participants were included in the process on a voluntary basis and informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary material 1., rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Gu, D., Jiang, J. Navigating an unpredictable environment: the moderating role of perceived environmental unpredictability in the effectiveness of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior. BMC Psychol 12 , 261 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01762-1

Download citation

Received : 01 December 2023

Accepted : 02 May 2024

Published : 10 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01762-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Ecological resource scarcity information
  • Pro-environmental behaviors
  • Life history theory
  • Washing-hands paradigm

BMC Psychology

ISSN: 2050-7283

psychology of terrorism research paper

ATTENTION: Our order fulfillment center is temporarily shutdown and unable to take orders. As a result, the Y24SAVE50 promotion is cancelled.

Yale University Press

On The Site

psychology of terrorism research paper

Photo by yeowatzup on Wikimedia Commons

  • How U.S. Policy in Yemen Went Tragically Wrong

May 13, 2024 | M'Baye, Fatou | Current Affairs , Middle East Studies , Politics

Alexandra Stark—

On October 8, 2016, an airstrike by the Saudi-led coalition struck a crowded funeral hall in Sanaa, Yemen, killing at least 140 people and wounding an additional 600, including children. The strike was shocking to many Americans, not only because of the tragic death toll, but because of how it happened: according to Human Rights Watch , remnants of a U.S.-manufactured bomb were identified at the site.

This was eight years after President Obama was elected with the promise that he would end the United States’ “ dumb wars ” in the Middle East. So how, and why, was a U.S.-made weapon contributing to the deaths of children in Yemen?

The answer is important—and not just to humanitarians who are concerned about preventing harm to Yemeni civilians. It’s important because it can help us to understand why even well-intentioned U.S. policy in the Middle East often goes so tragically wrong.

For decades, U.S. policy in Yemen has been shaped by narrowly defined U.S. security interests, from countering the Soviet Union’s influence in the region during the Cold War to combating terrorist organizations after 9/11. It was not about what could contribute to the well-being of Yemenis.

Before Houthi insurgents rose up and captured the capital in 2014, setting off the civil war, U.S. policy in Yemen was framed by its own counterterrorism interests. The United States faced the dilemma of how to fight Al Qaeda in the Arabian Peninsula (AQAP)—a terrorist organization linked to an attempted bombing on a U.S.-bound airliner in 2009 among other threats—without risking another quagmire in the Middle East. To effectively prevent terrorism without a larger U.S. investment, the U.S. partnered with Yemeni President Ali Abdullah Saleh’s government and launched the occasional drone or missile strike. At first, American officials thought this approach was so successful that they dubbed it “ the Yemen model ” and advertised its success through early 2015, when the country was overrun by civil war.

The Yemen model has not worked because this framework is too narrow. It meant that Obama administration officials were unable to see that their corrupt government partner had started to wobble, even before the Arab Spring protests washed over the country in 2011. And it meant that in March 2015, when Saudi Arabia asked for support in their military intervention against the Houthis, U.S. officials felt they had to go along. They wanted to maintain the United States’ important security relationship with Gulf partners, even though one official said that the decision felt like “getting into a car with a drunk driver.”

Later on, some U.S. officials would acknowledge that “we were wrong to think that cautious and at times conditional support for the war in Yemen would influence Saudi and Emirati policy.” Still, under the Trump administration, the U.S. doubled down on its support for the coalition until the October 2018 assassination of Saudi regime critic Jamal Khashoggi by Saudi agents.

In February 2021, President Biden announced that the United States would be “stepping up our diplomacy to end the war in Yemen,” and in April 2022, the UN managed to negotiate a truce . The truce has, more or less, held up even though the Houthis continue to attack commercial vessels in the Red Sea and U.S. still launches strikes on Houthi targets.

In the meantime, though, U.S. support had contributed to a war that caused “one of the world’s worst humanitarian crises,” according to Oxfam , causing over 12,000 civilian deaths and forcing almost 4 million people to flee the fighting. Eighty percent of Yemen’s population, or 24 million people, now relies on emergency aid. Aid groups have warned that the turmoil in the Red Sea could contribute to the ongoing humanitarian crisis and create additional barriers for the humanitarian organizations delivering lifesaving support in Yemen.

The lessons of the Yemen model therefore remain potent a decade on, as another devastating conflict rages in Gaza. A narrow focus on maintaining U.S. security partnerships, counter-terrorism operations, and countering Iranian influence did not achieve U.S. security goals in Yemen. What might a new approach look like? It could focus on the well-being of people in Yemen and across the region, instead of on only narrowly defined U.S. security interests.

Alexandra Stark  is an associate policy researcher at RAND, a nonprofit, nonpartian research institution, and a fellow at New America’s International Security Program. Her award-winning work has been published in academic and public outlets.

Recent Posts

psychology of terrorism research paper

  • The Naseby Cup: A Numismatic and Historical Treasure at Yale
  • Where Life Is, Hope Can Survive
  • A New Audience for the World’s First Author
  • Space Day Reading List 2024
  • The Musics of Michel Leiris

Sign up for updates on new releases and special offers

Newsletter signup, shipping location.

Our website offers shipping to the United States and Canada only. For customers in other countries:

Mexico and South America: Contact TriLiteral to place your order. All Others: Visit our Yale University Press London website to place your order.

Shipping Updated

Learn more about Schreiben lernen, 2nd Edition, available now. 

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Psychology of Terrorism

    psychology of terrorism research paper

  2. Psychology of Terrorism by Randy Borum

    psychology of terrorism research paper

  3. The Psychology of Terrorism

    psychology of terrorism research paper

  4. Psychology of Terrorism Research Paper

    psychology of terrorism research paper

  5. (PDF) Psychology of Terrorism

    psychology of terrorism research paper

  6. Sociological Critique of the Psychology of Terrorism Term Paper

    psychology of terrorism research paper

VIDEO

  1. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF TERRORISM- predictors, factors, research breakdown of my notes

  2. Impact of Terrorism in our Mental Health

  3. The famous 'Economic Terrorism' series by Zaid Hamid

  4. Narcissism || Terror & its Thrill as weapons || Rajat Mitra & Neeraj Atri

  5. Essay on TERRORISM || Terrorism essay || Paragraph on Terrorism || Terrorism paragraph || essay writ

  6. The Dark Psychology of Terrorism #darkpsychology #viral #mindcontrol #fyp

COMMENTS

  1. Psychology of terrorism: Introduction to the special issue.

    Abstract. Despite the extraordinary social and political consequences often associated with terrorist violence, as well as our responses to it, psychological research on terrorist behavior is conspicuously underdeveloped. This special issue of American Psychologist presents a series of articles that showcase new conceptual, theoretical, and ...

  2. PDF Psychology of Terrorism: Introduction to the Special Issue

    articles on terrorism research, Silke and Schmidt-Petersen (2015) found that the second and third (and then fifth and sixth) were research on psychological surveys of posttrau-Editor's note. This article introduces a collection published in a special issue of American Psychologist titled "Psychology of Terrorism" (April 2017).

  3. (PDF) The Psychology of Terrorism

    PDF | On Aug 6, 2020, Neil Shortland published The Psychology of Terrorism | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate

  4. Psychology of Terrorism

    This special issue of American Psychologist presents a series of articles that showcase new conceptual, theoretical, and empirical advances in our understanding of terrorism. In doing so, it seeks to not merely summarize recent accomplishments, but to highlight the immense value of explicitly psychological research on these issues, far more of ...

  5. Understanding the Terrorist Mind

    While early research focused on the political roots of terrorism, many of today's investigators are probing the psychological factors that drive adherents to commit their deadly deeds. ... Recent work in experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience provides some perspective on the mind of a terrorist. First, it is useful to define what ...

  6. Psychology of terrorism: Introduction to the special issue

    Abstract. Despite the extraordinary social and political consequences often associated with terrorist violence, as well as our responses to it, psychological research on terrorist behavior is conspicuously underdeveloped. This special issue of American Psychologist presents a series of articles that showcase new conceptual, theoretical, and ...

  7. Psychology of Terrorism: Introduction to the Special Issue

    The paper captures some of the trends in terrorist behaviour based on an analysis of demographic, historical, psychological and political factors. ... Research on the psychology of terrorism has ...

  8. Profiling of Terrorists' Psychologies

    This chapter traces the evolution of the four waves of modern terrorism and offers a typology of terrorism: anarchists, social revolutionaries, nationalist-separatists, religious fundamentalists, and lone-wolves. Each of these profiles of terrorism has its own characteristic psychology, which the chapter describes using real world examples.

  9. Terrorism Psychology: Theory & Application

    Since the 9/11 attacks on the World Trade Centre and the emergence of Al Qa'ida as a global terrorist threat, the theory and application of terrorism psychology has become a major interdisciplinary research area. With the intensification of home-grown terrorism at a time of resurgent levels of anti-Western rhetoric and global violence ...

  10. Psychology of Terrorism

    Psychology of Terrorism is an indispensable resource in the field of psychology and terrorism. Given the relative newness of the discipline, there is a great need for a text that covers relevant aspects of psychology related to terrorism. This online resource presents the newest findings on treatment and clinical response protocols.

  11. Terrorism, Psychology of

    Abstract. Terrorism is widely regarded as one of the most significant contemporary threats to global security. There continues to be significant disagreement, however, about how to define it. By the late 1980s, more than 100 definitions of terrorism had appeared in the professional literature. Criteria have often diverged across nations but are ...

  12. The Psychology of Terrorism: An Agenda for the 21st Century

    Future research should critically examine the assumption that a "new terrorism" has appeared at the end of the 20th century. Analysts should also take advantage of 30 years of history to develop comparisons and developmental studies that look not only at the causes of terrorism but at changes in terrorist strategy, the termination of ...

  13. [PDF] Psychological factors in terrorism and counterterrorism

    This article explores psychological factors involved in terrorism and counter-terrorism on individual, group, and organizational levels of analysis. On the individual level, we describe attempts to understand terrorist behavior as a form of psychopathology and/or as reflecting a unique constellation of personality traits. We also consider whether there exists a general motivational basis for ...

  14. Full article: Research on Terrorism, 2007-2016: A Review of Data

    Research on terrorism has long been criticized for its inability to overcome enduring methodological issues. ... 74.7% of research-based papers were the work of a single author and 74.8% of authors made only one such contribution to the field. ... (2017): 231-41; John G. Horgan, "Psychology of Terrorism: Introduction to the Special Issue ...

  15. Terrorism Research (2.5)

    This chapter sets out the field of terrorism studies and reviews the main issues and research directions that characterise the field today. The history of the discipline is summarised and terrorism and its 'near neighbour' hate crime are defined and compared before turning to the developments that have dominated the research agenda over the last ten years.

  16. (PDF) Psychology of Terrorism

    al., (2014) argue that the signi cance quest requires. three factors; (i) arousal of a goal of signi cance, (ii) identi cation of violence/terrorism as a means to sig. ni cance, and (iii ...

  17. (PDF) Kruglanski & Fishman (2006) The Psychology of Terrorism

    Research approaches leading to the understanding of the terrorist phenomenon are suggested. Seven principles explaining terrorism are analysed from a psychosocial approach: They could complete the explanations of terrorism focused in socio-structural and psychological variables, the latter being insufficient and often erroneous.

  18. Understanding terrorism

    Further research conducted by START co-director Kruglanski sheds light on the role a "collectivist mentality" may play in terrorism. His surveys of thousands of people in 15 Arab and other countries found that Muslims who have a more collectivistic mentality are more likely to support terrorist attacks against Americans than those with more ...

  19. PDF The Psychology of Terrorism: a Case Study of Osama Bin Laden

    THE PSYCHOLOGY OF TERRORISM: A CASE STUDY OF OSAMA BIN LADEN A Thesis submitted to the Faculty of The School of Continuing Studies and of The Graduate School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Liberal Studies By Juliana Gibson Steiner, B.A. Georgetown University Washington, D.C.

  20. PDF Psychology of Terrorism

    • Research on the psychology of terrorism largely lacks substance and rigor. Cultural factors are important, but have not been studied. Future research should be operationally-informed; maintain ... The purpose of this paper is to analyze and synthesize what has been reported from the scientific and professional literature about the

  21. PDF The Sociology and Psychology of Terrorism: Who becomes a ...

    This product was prepared by the staff of the Federal Research Division of the Library of Congress under an Interagency Agreement with the sponsoring United States Government agency. The Federal Research Division is the Library of Congress's primary fee-for-service research unit and has served United States Government agencies since 1948. At the

  22. Full article: How Did 9/11 Affect Terrorism Research? Examining

    The post-9/11 average through 2019 is 304 articles per year. Several additional points are apparent. First, the post-9/11 increase started in 2001—in spite of the attacks occurring in September, and time required peer review. 2001 saw 168 articles on terrorism, mostly in the final months of the year.

  23. Terrorism

    Terrorism is a label that is not applied evenly; the rhetorical processes at play in labelling an individual or a group as terrorist are influenced by major stakeholders, for example government, the media, think tanks, etc. Psychology as a discipline has long been seeking to answer the question of why people get involved in terrorism ...

  24. Understanding Counter-terrorism Strategies of Police Against the

    In this paper, we examine the roles of Bangladesh Police in fighting terrorism in the country. In terms of theory, the starting point is the concept of counter-terrorism. The researchers investigate the efforts of police in combating terrorism against the changing dynamics of terrorism in Bangladesh also in the absence of a national counter-terrorism strategy. In addition to consulting ...

  25. Impact of terrorism on child sex at birth: evidence from Pakistan

    Using insights from the literature on psychology and medicine, we examine the impact of stress induced by terrorism on child sex at birth. The psychological and social stressors associated with terrorist events prior to conception may trigger changes in parental hormones that have an implication for birth outcomes. We extract data on 11,331 live births conceived between 2007 and 2012 from ...

  26. Evaluating and Assessing Terrorism Prevention Programs: What Research

    Important lessons that emerged from the research underscore the importance of gaining community buy-in and assuring program relevance for participants before implementing the programs; program benefits can extend beyond meeting terrorism prevention goals and can be carried out within public health or community resilience initiatives; and ...

  27. Navigating an unpredictable environment: the moderating role of

    Background The global issue of ecological resource scarcity, worsened by climate change, necessitates effective methods to promote resource conservation. One commonly used approach is presenting ecological resource scarcity information. However, the effectiveness of this method remains uncertain, particularly in an unpredictable world. This research aims to examine the role of perceived ...

  28. How U.S. Policy in Yemen Went Tragically Wrong

    To effectively prevent terrorism without a larger U.S. investment, the U.S. partnered with Yemeni President Ali Abdullah Saleh's government and launched the occasional drone or missile strike. At first, American officials thought this approach was so successful that they dubbed it " the Yemen model " and advertised its success through ...

  29. Thinking outside of the (Western) Box: Cultural Psychology Perspectives

    The study mentioned in the paper was financed, in part, by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (translation Higher Education Personnel Improvement Coordination)—Brazil (CAPES)—Finance Code 001, which awarded the first author a scholarship to work with the third author at Dublin City University that, in turn, facilitated the collaboration for this paper.