• Browse All Articles
  • Newsletter Sign-Up

SocialPsychology →

No results found in working knowledge.

  • Were any results found in one of the other content buckets on the left?
  • Try removing some search filters.
  • Use different search filters.

Current Research in Social Psychology

Editors: michael lovaglia, university of iowa; shane soboroff, st. ambrose university.

Current Research in Social Psychology  ( CRISP ) is a peer reviewed, electronic journal publishing theoretically driven, empirical research in major areas of social psychology. Publication is sponsored by the  Center for the Study of Group Processes  at the  University of Iowa,  which provides free access to its contents. Authors retain copyright for their work. CRISP is permanently archived at the Library of the University of Iowa and at the Library of Congress. Beginning in April, 2000,  Sociological Abstracts  publishes the abstracts of CRISP articles.

Citation Format:  Lastname ,  Firstname . 1996. "Title of Article."  Current Research in Social Psychology  2:15-22 https://crisp.org.uiowa.edu

RECENT ISSUES

Finding Positives in the Pandemic: The Role of Relationship Status, Self-Esteem, Mental Health, and Personality.

Examining Public Attitudes And Ideological Divides Through Media Engagement: An Empirical Analysis of Moral Foundations Theory Amidst the Covid-19 Pandemic.

When Race is Not Enough: Lessons Learned Using Racially Tagged Names.

Formation of a Positive Social Identity: How Significant are Attitudes, Subjective Norms, and Perceived Similarity Concerning Group Identification?

Passive Social Network Usage and Hedonic Well-Being Among Vietnamese University Students: A Moderated Mediation Model Involving Self-Esteem and Sense of Self.

Cognitive Dissonance and Depression: A Qualitative Exploration of a Close Relationship.

Gender Differences in Support for Collective Punishment: The Moderating Role of Malleability Mindset.

Hard Feelings? Predicting Attitudes Toward Former Romantic Partners.

Perceived Control in Multiple Option Scenarios: Choice, Control, and the Make-a-Difference Metric.

Drivers of Prosocial Behavior: Exploring the Role of Mindset and Perceived Cost.

Malleability of Laïcité: People with High Social Dominance Orientation Use Laïcité to Legitimize Public Prayer by Catholics but not by Muslims.

Differences and Predictive Abilities of Competitiveness Between Motivation Levels, Contexts, and Sex.

Parental Rejection and Peer Acceptance: The Mediating Role of Cognitive Bias.

A Novel Approach for Measuring Self-Affirmation.

Ingroup Bias in the Context of Meat Consumption: Direct and Indirect Attitudes Toward Meat-Eaters and Vegetarians.

Perceptions of Case Complexity and Pre-Trial Publicity Through the Lens of Information Processing.

"Muslims' Desire for Intergroup Revenge in the Aftermath of the Christchurch Attack: The Predictive Role of Ingroup Identification, Perceived Intergroup Threat, and the Norm of Reciprocity. "

"Personal Networks and Social Support in Disaster Contexts."

"Aggressive and Avoidant Action Tendencies Towards Out-Groups: The Distinct Roles of In-Group Attachment vs. Glorification and Cognitive vs. Affective Ambivalence."

"We (Might) Want You: Expectations of Veterans' General Competence and Leadership."

"Situation Attribution Mediates Intention to Overlook Negative Signals Among Romantic Partners."

"Software Program, Bot, or Artificial Intelligence? Affective Sentiments across General Technology Labels"

"Privilege is Invisible to Those Who Have It": Some Evidence that Men Underestimate the Magnitude of Gender Differences in Income.

"Perceived Control and Intergroup Discrimination."  

"Leadership, Gender, and Vocal Dynamics in Small Groups."

Taking Responsibility for an Offense: Being Forgiven Encourages More Personal Responsibility, More Empathy for the Victim, and Less Victim Blame.

Potential Factors Influencing Attitudes Toward Veterans Who Commit Crimes: An Experimental Investigation of PTSD in the Legal System.

"Is that Discrimination? I'd Better Report it!" Self-presentation Concerns Moderate the Prototype Effect.

Relation Between Attitudinal Trust and Behavioral Trust: An Exploratory Study

Comparing Groups' Affective Sentiments to Group Perceptions.

Perceived Autonomous Help and Recipients' Well-Being: Is Autonomous Help Good for Everyone.

S tudying Gay and Straight Males' Implicit Gender Attitudes to Understand Previously Found Gender Differences in Implicit In-Group Bias.

Nepotistic Preferences in a Computerized Trolley Problem.

Telecommuting, Primary Caregiving, and Gender as Status .

You're Either With Us or Against Us: In-Group Favoritism and Threat .

 Impact of the Anticipation of Membership Change on Transactive Memory and Group Performance.

Mindfulness Increases Analytical Thought and Decreases Just World Beliefs .

Status, Performance Expectations, and Affective Impressions: An Experimental Replication.

The Effects of African-American Stereotype Fluency on Prejudicial Evaluation of Targets .

Status Characteristics and Self-Categoriation: A Bridge Across theoretical Traditions.

Why do Extraverts Feel More Positive Affect and Life Satisfaction? The Indirect Effects of Social Contribution and Sense of Power.

In-group Attachment and Glorification, Perceptions of Cognition-Based Ambivalence as Contributing to the Group, and Positive Affect.

Mentoring to Improve a Child's Self-Concept: Longitudinal Effects of Social Intervention on Identity and Negative Outcomes.

Affect, Emotion, and Cross-Cultural Differences in Moral Attributions.

The Effects of Counterfactual Thinking on College Students' Intentions to Quit Smoking Cigarettes .

Self-Enhancement, Self-Protection and Ingroup Bias.

The Moderating Effect of Socio-emotional Factors on the Relationship Between Status and Influence in Status Characteristics Theory.

What We Know About People Shapes the Inferences We Make About Their Personalities.

The Pros and Cons of Ingroup Ambivalence: The Moderating Roles of Attitudinal Basis and Individual Differences in Ingroup Attachment and Glorification.

Effects of Social Anxiety and Group Membership of Potential Affiliates on Social Reconnection After Ostracism.

"Yes, I Decide You Will Recieve Your Choice": Effects of Authoritative Agreement on Perceptions of Control.

Being Generous to Look Good: Perceived Stigma Increases Prosocial Behavior in Smokers.

Acting White? Black Young Adults Devalue Same-Race Targets for Demonstrating Positive-but-Stereotypically White Traits

Looking Up for Answers: Upward Gaze Increases Receptivity to Advice

Which Judgement Do Women Expect from a Female Observer When They Claim to be a Victim of Sexism?

Neighborhood Deterioration and Perceptions of Race

The Use of Covert and Overt Jealousy Tactics in Romantic Relationships: The Moderating Role of Relationship Satisfaction

The Impact of Status Differences on Gatekeeping: A Theoretical Bridge and Bases for Investigation

Reducing Prejudice with (Elaborated) Imagined and Physical Intergroup Contact Interverventions

Are Depressed Individuals More Susceptible to Cognitive Dissonance?

Gender Differences in the Need to Belong: Different Cognitive Representations of the Same Social Groups

Fight The Power: Comparing and Evaluating Two Measures of French and Raven's (1959) Bases of Social Power

Mother Knows Best So Mother Fails Most: Benevolent Stereotypes and the Punishment of Parenting Mistakes

Blame Attributions about Disloyalty

Attitudes Towards Muslims are More Favorable on a Survery than on an Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure

Attributions to Low Group Effort can Make You Feel Better: The Distinct Roles of In-group Identification, Legitimacy of Intergroup Status, and Controllability Perceptions

The Role of Collective and Personal Self-Esteem in a Military Context

On Bended Knee: Embodiment and Religious Judgments

Identity Salience and Identity Importance in Identity Theory

Sexist Humor and Beliefs that Justify Societal Sexism

Future-Oriented People Show Stronger Moral Concerns

Further Examining the Buffering Effect of Self-Esteem and Mastery on Emotions

Group-Based Resiliency: Contrasting the Negative Effects of Threat to the In-Group

You Validate Me, You Like Me, You're Fun, You Expand Me: "I'm Yours!"

Pleading Innocents: Laboratory Evidence of Plea Bargaining's Innocence Problem

The Moral Identity and Group Affiliation

Threat, Prejudice, and Stereotyping in the Context of Japanese, North Korean, and South Korean Intergroup Relations

Exams may be Dangerous to Grandpa's Health: How Inclusive Fitness Influences Students' Fraudulent Excuses

To View Archived CRISP Issues Click here

To View the Notice for Contributors Click here . Includes formatting and citation guidelines.

To View the Editorial Board Click here

  • Customers outside USA/CAN
  • Forgot password
  • Cart article/s To Cart

Testzentrale

Social psychology.

  • ISSN L: 1864-9335
  • ISSN Print: 1864-9335
  • ISSN Online: 2151-2590

About the journal

For authors.

  • More information

Editors & editorial board

Order information, read online.

  • Advance articles
  • Current issue
  • Open access
  • Free online sample issue

Social Psychology publishes innovative and methodologically sound research and serves as an international forum for scientific discussion and debate in the field of social psychology. Topics include all basic social psychological research themes, methodological advances in social psychology, as well as research in applied fields of social psychology. The journal focuses on original empirical contributions to social psychological research, but is open to theoretical articles, critical reviews, and replications of published research. 

The journal was published until volume 38 (2007) as the Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie (ISSN 0044-3514). Drawing on nearly 50 years of experience and tradition in publishing high-quality, innovative science , Social Psychology has an internationally renowned team of editors and consulting editors from all areas of basic and applied social psychology, thus ensuring that the highest international standards are maintained. 

Social Psychology offers a transparent peer-review process and a short time-lag between acceptance of papers and publication.

Social Psychology is archived with Portico .

Seeking nominations for the Editor-in-Chief position Hogrefe Publishing is soliciting nominations – including self-nominations – for the post of editor-in-chief for the journal Social Psychology , starting September 2024 when the term of the current editor-in-chief ends. Please see the published call for nominations for details.

New additional editors in chief join the journal Social Psychology Hogrefe Publishing and the editorial office of Social Psychology are delighted that Prof. Michael Häfner, PhD (Berlin), and Prof. Mario Gollwitzer, PhD (Munich), have agreed to join the journal on a temporary basis as additional editors in chief, starting in March 2024. The term of office for Editor in Chief Prof. Anja Achtziger, PhD, is due to end September 2024, and so a call for nominations to find a new, longer-term solution from September 2024 will, as planned, be released in the next few weeks. Click here for more information.

Open Access for UK authors publishing in APA PsycArticles Hogrefe is participating in the open access publishing pilot for the APA PsycArticles database in 2023-24 – this includes eligible articles accepted in Social Psychology. Learn more

We are looking forward to receiving your manuscript for Social Psychology . Please read and take note of the instructions to authors before submitting your manuscript. All manuscripts should be submitted via Editorial Manager . Thank you!

Useful links:

  • Guidelines on sharing and use of articles in Hogrefe journals
  • APA style tutorials
  • OpenMind – Hogrefe’s open access program
  • Authors from UK – please see the information about open access publication under our read-and-publish agreement, in collaboration with APA and Jisc
  • Authors from Germany – please read our FAQs about Plan S compliant open access publication in our journals under a publish-and-read agreement with 100+ institutions
  • Instructions to authors (PDF, 223 KB)
  • Title page template (DOCX, 53 KB)

Publication Ethics

It is important to the Hogrefe Publishing Group that our scientific journals and all the people involved adhere to the highest ethical standards. Please take a moment to review our guidelines on what this means for authors, editors, reviewers, and us as a publisher.

research article social psychology

Editing and translation services

Hogrefe has negotiated a 20% discount for authors who wish to have their manuscript professionally edited or translated into English by the experts at Enago before submission. Please note that the service is independent of Hogrefe and use of it has no bearing on acceptance decisions made by individual journals.

Social Psychology is on X (formerly known as Twitter)!

research article social psychology

The editors set up an account for the journal to keep you posted about the latest news and research in the field of social psychology. 

Recent editorial

“Understanding Others in Moments of Crisis” A Special Issue of Social Psychology Dana Schneider, Pascal Burgmer, Thorsten M. Erle, and Heather Ferguson Social Psychology, Vol. 54, No. 1-2, pp. 1-3

research article social psychology

Anja Achtziger

Editor-in-chief

Am Seemooser Horn 20

research article social psychology

Mario Gollwitzer

Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München

Department Psychologie

Lehrstuhl für Sozialpsychologie

Leopoldstraße 13

research article social psychology

Michael Häfner

Professor für Kommunikationspsychologie

Universität der Künste Berlin

Postfach 120544

Editorial office

Alexander Jaudas Zeppelin Universität Friedrichshafen Am Seemooser Horn 20 88045 Friedrichshafen Germany Tel.: +49 7541 6009 1372 Send email

Associate editors

Wing Yee (VerBon) Cheung, University of Winchester, UK Send email

Adam K. Fetterman, University of Houston, USA Send email

Teresa Garcia-Marques, ISPA Instituto Universitário, Portugal Send email

Jaume Garcia-Segarra, Universitat Jaume I, Spain Send email

Andreas Mojzisch, University of Hildesheim, Germany Send email

Lukas Thürmer, University of Salzburg, Austria Send email

Editorial Board

Susanne Abele, Oxford, OH, USA Andrea Abele-Brehm, Erlangen, Germany Anna Baumert, Munich, Germany Herbert Bless, Mannheim, Germany Gerd Bohner, Bielefeld, Germany Oliver Christ, Hagen, Germany Paul Conway, Portsmouth, UK Katja Corcoran, Graz, Austria Olivier Corneille, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Amanda Diekman, Bloomington, IN, USA Andrew Elliott, Rorchester, NY, USA Kai Epstude, Groningen, The Netherlands Bertram Gawronski, Austin, TX, USA Guido Gendolla, Geneva, Switzerland Jessica Good, Davidson, NC, USA Tobias Greitemeyer, Innsbruck, Austria Michael Häfner, Berlin, Germany Bettina Hannover, Berlin, Germany Nina Hansen, Groningen, The Netherlands Nicole Harth, Jena, Germany S. Alexander Haslam, Brisbane, Australia Ying-yi Hong, Hong Kong, ROC Joachim Hüffmeier, Dortmund, Germany Roland Imhoff, Mainz, Germany Eva Jonas, Salzburg, Austria Franciska Krings, Lausanne, Switzerland Toon Kuppens, Groningen, The Netherlands Maja Kutlaca, Osnabrueck, Germany Daniel Lakens, Eindhoven, The Netherlands Thomas Morton, Exeter, UK Corinne Moss-Racusin, Saratoga Springs, NY, USA Roland Neumann, Trier, Germany Esther Papies, Glasgow, UK Susanne Quadflieg, Bristol, UK Marc-André Reinhard, Kassel, Germany Toni Schmader, Vancouver, BC, Canada Manfred Schmitt, Landau, Germany Thomas W. Schubert, Oslo, Norway Beate Seibt, Oslo, Norway Monika Sieverding, Heidelberg, Germany Dagmar Stahlberg, Mannheim, Germany Fritz Strack, Würzburg, Germany Christian Unkelbach, Cologne, Germany Rolf van Dick, Frankfurt/Main, Germany Harm Veling, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Tobias Vogel, Mannheim, Germany Michaele Wänke, Mannheim, Germany Ulrich Wagner, Marburg, Germany Eva Walther, Trier, Germany Michael Wohl, Ottawa, ON, Canada Bogdan Wojciszke, Warsaw, Poland Kumar Yogeeswaran, Canterbury, New Zealand Vincent Yzerbyt, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium

Social Psychology is published bimonthly, online.

Subscriptions generally run from January to December.

For individual and institutional customers, online access to the subscribed journal is included in the annual subscription rate. The online access is valid for the duration of the subscription.

Further information on our subscription conditions can be found in the Information for subscribers or in our current journals catalog .

Individuals 2024

Single issue 2024.

Single online issues may be purchased online at  Hogrefe eContent. Single print copies of back issues (up to 06/2023) may be ordered (subject to availability) using this order enquiry form. Please specify the desired volume number and issue number when ordering.

Institutions 2024

Prices for institutions depend on the size of the institution and can be found in the table below. Further information and prices for journal subscriptions can be found in our journals catalog – or contact us via email for a price quote.

Prices for institutions

whatsapp

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

The 9 Major Research Areas in Social Psychology

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

research article social psychology

Mitchell Funk / Getty Images

Social Cognition

Violence and aggression, prosocial behavior, prejudice and discrimination.

  • Social Identity

Group Behavior

Social influence, interpersonal relationships.

Social psychology is a branch of psychology that studies a wide range of subjects related to social behavior. This includes studying how people interact, factors that affect social perceptions, the formation of attitudes, and how groups influence individuals.

Research in social psychology is often focused on subjects that fall within three broad areas:

  • Social influence : Social influence refers to the ways in which our opinions and behavior are affected by the presence of others. This includes studies on topics such as conformity, obedience, and social pressure.
  • Social perception : Social perception refers to the ways in which we form impressions of other people. This includes research on topics including first impressions, stereotyping, and prejudice.
  • Social interaction : Social interaction refers to the ways in which we interact with other people. This includes research on topics such as communication, aggression, and altruism.

This article discusses some of the major areas of research in social psychology. It also explores some examples of the types of research that social psychologists might conduct within these subject areas.

Social cognition is concerned with the processing, storage, and application of social information. For example, research in this area of social psychology may focus on the development and use of social schemas. 

Schemas are our general ideas about the world, how things are, and how things work. In the case of social schemas, these ideas relate to how we expect people to behave in different situations.

These mental categories allow us to function without constantly stopping to interpret everything around us. We also develop associations between related schemas, which play an important role in the thought process and social behavior.

One area of social cognition research concerns person perception , which is how people form impressions of others. 

First impressions are the judgments we form about someone based on limited information. Studies have shown that first impressions happen within mere milliseconds and are based on several cues, such as facial expressions, body language, voice, and the beliefs held by the observer.  

Understanding how people acquire and process social information allows researchers to better explain how it can affect social interactions and individual behavior.

Attitudes and Attitude Change

Another major research area in social psychology involves the study of attitudes . Social psychologists are interested in the components of attitudes, how attitudes develop, and how attitudes change.

Attitudes are evaluations of people, objects, or issues. They can be positive (e.g., "I like chocolate") or negative (e.g., "I dislike taxes"). Various factors contribute to the development of attitudes, including upbringing and experiences, although genetics also appears to play a role in shaping them.

Researchers have identified three core components of attitude: an affective component, a behavioral component, and a cognitive component. Often referred to as the "ABCs of attitude," these elements describe how we feel, behave, and understand.

Some other characteristics of attitudes that researchers may be interested in include:

  • How they are best measured : Some attitudes can be measured through self-report questionnaires, but others might be better measured using tools like facial expression or arousal levels.
  • Factors that affect their strength : Attitudes can vary considerably in terms of their intensity. The strength of these attitudes directly impacts the degree to which they will guide their actions. Direct experiences and frequent exposure to the attitude can impact its strength.
  • How attitudes affect behavior : Researchers are also interested in understanding how and when these attitudes influence people's actions. For example, social psychologists might explore how attitudes develop through exposure to social media sources and how those attitudes relate to real-world actions.

Attitudes are an important research topic in social psychology because they impact how people view and interact with others.

What causes violence and aggression ? While many different factors play a role, social psychologists are interested in understanding the social influences that shape violent behavior.

Research in this area looks at numerous social factors that may cause aggression, including:

  • Situational variables that might contribute to aggression
  • Non-physical types of aggression such as name-calling or gossiping
  • How aggression is learned via modeling, such as witnessing adults or children engage in aggressive or violent behaviors
  • How violence in the media affects behavior in the real world
  • Strategies that can be effective in the reduction of aggression and violence
  • The role social learning plays in producing aggressive behaviors and actions
  • How public policy can be used to curb violent behavior

Research into the epidemic of gun violence is an example of how social psychologists are trying to understand the variables that contribute to a problem, and then utilize that knowledge to come up with actionable solutions.

Prosocial behavior is another major research area in social psychology. Prosocial behaviors are those that involve helping and cooperating.

Researchers often look at why people help others, as well as why they sometimes refuse to help or cooperate. The bystander effect is an example of a social phenomenon in the subject area of prosocial behavior.

Much of the research in the area of bystander effect was prompted by the murder of a young woman named Kitty Genovese. This case captured national attention when reports suggested that neighbors had witnessed her attack and murder but failed to call the police for help.

Later reviews of the case indicate that few (if any) of the neighbors had a clear view of the scene and were unaware of what was happening. Nevertheless, the case became mythologized in psychology textbooks and prompted a surge of interest in prosocial behaviors.

Research inspired by the Genovese case produced a great deal of information on prosocial behavior and how and why people choose—or sometimes refuse—to help others.

Prejudice, discrimination, and stereotypes exist in any social group. Social psychologists are interested in the origins, causes, and effects of these attitudes and social categorizations.

Some questions that social psychologists explore include:

  • How does prejudice develop?
  • Why are stereotypes maintained in the face of contrary evidence?
  • How can prejudice be measured?
  • What factors contribute to the formation of prejudice and discrimination?
  • Are there effective ways to reduce prejudice and discrimination?

For example, researchers have found that several factors contribute to the development of prejudice, including stereotypes, social categorization, and social influences. Another factor that plays a part is the outgroup homogeneity bias, or the tendency to view people outside of our social group as being more homogenous than members of our own group.

By learning more about the psychology of prejudice and discrimination, researchers can look for solutions to help help prevent it from happening.

Self and Social Identity

Our perceptions of social identities and ourselves are another important research area in social psychology. Some of the questions that researchers explore include:

  • How do people come to know and understand themselves?
  • How do these self-perceptions affect our social interactions?
  • How does belonging to different social groups shape individual identity?
  • How do intersecting group members influence self-perception and self-identity?

Social psychologists are interested in learning more about how this inner life influences our outer lives and social world. Self-awareness, self-esteem, self-concept , and self-expression are only a few factors that influence our social experience.

For example, social comparison is a process that can impact how people view themselves. Upward social comparison involves comparing the self to others who are perceived as higher in status and ability, while downward social comparison focuses on making comparisons to those who are lower in status or ability.

Upward comparisons can leave people feeling like they don't measure up, damaging self-esteem. Downward comparisons, on the other hand, can help enhance self-esteem.

By learning more about how social identities and self-perceptions interact, social psychologists are better able to understand how social factors can influence how individuals feel about themselves and their identities.

Group behavior is defined as the actions, feelings, or thoughts of a collective of people. Such groups involve two or more people who share something in common such as identity, purpose, and belief.

The behavior of groups is one of the largest research areas in social psychology. Most people realize that groups tend to behave differently than individuals. These group behaviors are sometimes beneficial but can also be detrimental.

Social psychologists often look at topics such as:

  • Group dynamics
  • Group decision making
  • Cooperation
  • Group influence

Norms are an example of an aspect of group behavior that can guide how group members think, behave, or act. Norms are standards that emerge and guide how another member judge one another.

Social psychologists are also interested in the role of social influence on behavior and decision-making. Topics such as the psychology of persuasion , peer pressure, conformity , and obedience are only a few of those studied in this area of social psychology.

One example of research in this area of social psychology was Milgram's obedience studies conducted during the 1960s. The experiments found that when ordered by an authority figure, participants were willing to deliver what they believed were dangerous and painful electrical shocks to another person. While the shocks were staged, the research suggested that many people were willing to go to great lengths to obey authority.

Research has helped reveal the power of social influence and has uncovered ways to help people resist influence.

Social relationships play a major role in shaping behavior, attitudes, feelings, and thoughts. Social psychologists study how these interpersonal relationships affect people by looking at attachment , liking , love , and attraction.

Some research questions that social psychologists might explore include:

  • How important are interpersonal relationships to individual well-being?
  • What factors play a role in attraction?
  • How do interpersonal relationships influence helping behaviors in groups?
  • How do close relationships affect individuals?

Close relationships are relationships in which we feel a strong sense of connection and intimacy with another person. Studies on close relationships have shown that they are associated with many benefits, such as increased happiness and satisfaction with life.

A Word From Verywell

Social psychology is a rich subject that explores how social perception, social interaction, and social influence affect both groups and individuals. Researchers in this field are interested in various topics, including attitudes, attraction, close relationships, and helping behavior. By learning more about these subjects, social psychologists can add to our understanding of social behavior and its effect on individual well-being.

Venta A, Hatkevich C, Mellick W, Vanwoerden S, Sharp C. Social cognition mediates the relation between attachment schemas and posttraumatic stress disorder . Psychol Trauma. 2017;9(1):88-95. doi:10.1037/tra0000165

Stolier RM, Hehman E, Keller MD, Walker M, Freeman JB. The conceptual structure of face impressions . Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A . 2018;115(37):9210-9215. doi:10.1073/pnas.1807222115

Markovitch N, Netzer L, Tamir M. Will you touch a dirty diaper? Attitudes towards disgust and behaviour [published correction appears in Cogn Emot . 2016;30(3):i].  Cogn Emot . 2016;30(3):592–602. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1020049

Olson JM, Vernon PA, Harris JA, Jang KL. The heritability of attitudes: A study of twins . J Pers Soc Psychol . 2001;80(6):845-60. PMID: 11414369.

Van Ryzin MJ, Dishion TJ. From antisocial behavior to violence: a model for the amplifying role of coercive joining in adolescent friendships .  J Child Psychol Psychiatry . 2013;54(6):661–669. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12017

Kassin SM. The killing of Kitty Genovese: What else does this case tell us?   Perspect Psychol Sci . 2017;12(3):374–381. doi:10.1177/1745691616679465

Rhodes M, Mandalaywala TM. The development and developmental consequences of social essentialism .  Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci . 2017;8(4):10.1002/wcs.1437. doi:10.1002/wcs.1437

Hjerm M, Eger M, Danell R.  Peer attitudes and the development of prejudice in adolescence .  Socius Sociolog Res Dynamic World . 2018;4:1-11. doi:10.1177/2378023118763187

American Psychological Association.  Outgroup homogeneity bias .

Drury J, Carter H, Cocking C, Ntontis E, Tekin Guven S, Amlôt R. Facilitating collective psychosocial resilience in the public in emergencies: Twelve recommendations based on the social identity approach [published correction appears in Front Public Health . 2019 Jun 27;7:181].  Front Public Health . 2019;7:141. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2019.00141

Rahman T. Extreme Overvalued Beliefs: How Violent Extremist Beliefs Become "Normalized" .  Behav Sci (Basel) . 2018;8(1):10. doi:10.3390/bs8010010

Russell NJC.  Milgram's obedience to authority experiments: Origins and early evolution .  Br J Soc Psychol . 2011;50:140-162. doi:10.1348/014466610X492205

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 07 May 2024

Mechanisms linking social media use to adolescent mental health vulnerability

  • Amy Orben   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-2937-4183 1 ,
  • Adrian Meier   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-8191-2962 2 ,
  • Tim Dalgleish   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-7304-2231 1 &
  • Sarah-Jayne Blakemore 3 , 4  

Nature Reviews Psychology ( 2024 ) Cite this article

3828 Accesses

123 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Psychiatric disorders
  • Science, technology and society

Research linking social media use and adolescent mental health has produced mixed and inconsistent findings and little translational evidence, despite pressure to deliver concrete recommendations for families, schools and policymakers. At the same time, it is widely recognized that developmental changes in behaviour, cognition and neurobiology predispose adolescents to developing socio-emotional disorders. In this Review, we argue that such developmental changes would be a fruitful focus for social media research. Specifically, we review mechanisms by which social media could amplify the developmental changes that increase adolescents’ mental health vulnerability. These mechanisms include changes to behaviour, such as sharing risky content and self-presentation, and changes to cognition, such as modifications in self-concept, social comparison, responsiveness to social feedback and experiences of social exclusion. We also consider neurobiological mechanisms that heighten stress sensitivity and modify reward processing. By focusing on mechanisms by which social media might interact with developmental changes to increase mental health risks, our Review equips researchers with a toolkit of key digital affordances that enables theorizing and studying technology effects despite an ever-changing social media landscape.

You have full access to this article via your institution.

Similar content being viewed by others

research article social psychology

Loneliness trajectories over three decades are associated with conspiracist worldviews in midlife

research article social psychology

Determinants of behaviour and their efficacy as targets of behavioural change interventions

research article social psychology

Adults who microdose psychedelics report health related motivations and lower levels of anxiety and depression compared to non-microdosers

Introduction.

Adolescence is a period marked by profound neurobiological, behavioural and environmental changes that facilitate the transition from familial dependence to independent membership in society 1 , 2 . This critical developmental stage is also characterized by diminished well-being and increased vulnerability to the onset of mental health conditions 3 , 4 , 5 , particularly socio-emotional disorders such as depression, and eating disorders 4 , 6 (Fig. 1 ). Notable symptoms of socio-emotional disorders include heightened negative affect, mood dysregulation and an increased focus on distress or challenges concerning interpersonal relationships, including heightened sensitivity to peers or perceptions of others 6 . Although some risk factors for socio-emotional disorders do not necessarily occur in adolescence (including genetic predispositions, adverse childhood experiences and poverty 7 , 8 , 9 ), the unique developmental characteristics of this period of life can interact with pre-existing vulnerabilities, increasing the risk of disorder onset 10 .

figure 1

Meta-analytic proportion of age of onset of anxiety (red), obsessive-compulsive disorder (purple), eating disorders (orange), personality disorders (green), schizophrenia (grey) and mood disorders (blue). The peak age of onset (dotted lines) is 5.5 and 15.5 years for anxiety, 14.5 years for obsessive-compulsive disorder, 15.5 years for eating disorders and 20.5 years for personality disorders, schizophrenia and mood disorders. Adapted from ref. 258 , CC BY 4.0 ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ).

Over the past decade, declines in adolescent mental health have become a great concern 11 , 12 . The prevalence of socio-emotional disorders has increased in the adolescent age range (10–24 years 2 ) 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 21 , leading to mounting pressures on child and adolescent mental health services 16 , 21 , 22 . This increase has not been as pronounced among other age groups when compared with adolescents 20 , 22 , 23 (measured in ref.  20 , ref.  22 and ref.  23 as age 12–25 years, 12–20 years and 18–25 years, respectively), even if some studies have found increases across the entire lifespan 24 , 25 . Although these trends might not be generalizable across the world 26 or to subclinical indicators of distress 15 , similar trends have been found in a range of countries 27 . Declines in adolescent mental health, especially socio-emotional problems, are consistent across datasets and researchers have argued that they are not solely driven by changes in social attitudes, stigma or reporting of distress 28 , 29 .

Concurrently, adolescents’ lives have become increasingly digital, with most young people using social media platforms throughout the day 30 . Ninety-five per cent of UK adolescents aged 15 years use social media 31 , and 50% of US adolescents aged 13–17 years report being almost constantly online 32 . The social media environment impacts adolescent and adult life across many domains (for example, by enabling social communication or changing the way news is accessed) and influences individuals, dyads and larger social systems 33 , 34 , 35 , 36 . Because social media is inherently social and relational 37 , it potentially overlaps and interacts with the developmental changes that make adolescents vulnerable to the onset of mental health problems 38 , 39 (Fig. 2 ). Thus, it has been intensely debated whether the increase in social media use during the past decade has a causal role in the decline of adolescent mental health 40 . Indeed, rapid changes to the environment experienced before and during adolescence might be a fruitful area to explore when examining current mental health trends 41 .

figure 2

During adolescence, the interaction between genetic programming (yellow), social determinants (red) and environmental factors (blue), as well as the developmental changes discussed in this Review, increases the risk for onset of mental health conditions. Digital environments, mediated behaviours and experiences, and the impact that this technology has on society and economy more generally, are one aspect of the complex forces that might lead to the declines in adolescent mental health observed in the last decade. Adapted from ref. 259 , Springer Nature Limited.

Although there are many environmental changes that could be relevant, a substantial body of research has emerged to investigate the potential link between social media use and declines in adolescent mental health 42 , 43 using various research approaches, including cross-sectional studies 44 , longitudinal observational data analyses 45 , 46 , 47 and experimental studies 48 , 49 . However, the scientific results have been mixed and inconclusive (for reviews, see refs. 43 , 50 , 51 , 52 , 53 ), which has made it difficult to establish evidence-based recommendations, regulations and interventions aimed at ensuring that social media use is not harmful to adolescents 54 , 55 , 56 , 57 .

Many researchers attribute the mixed results to insufficient study specificity. For instance, the relationship between social media use and mental health varies notably across individuals 45 , 58 and developmental time windows 59 . Yet studies often examine adolescents without differentiating them based on age or developmental stage 60 , which prevents systematic accounts of individual and subgroup differences. Additionally, most studies only rely on self-reported measures of time spent on social media 61 , 62 , and overlook more nuanced aspects of social media use such as the nature of the activities 63 and the content or features that users engage with 52 . These factors need to be considered to unpack any broader relationships 35 , 64 , 65 , 66 . Furthermore, the measurement of mental health often conflates positive and negative mental health outcomes as well as various mental health conditions, which could all be differentially related to social media use 52 , 67 .

This research space presents substantial complexity 68 . There is an ever-increasing range of potential combinations of social media predictors, well-being and mental health outcomes and participant groups of varying backgrounds and demographics that can become the target of scientific investigation. However, the pressure to deliver policy and public-facing recommendations and interventions leaves little time to investigate comprehensively each of these combinations. Researchers need to be able to pinpoint quickly the research programmes with the maximum potential to create translational and real-world impact for adolescent mental health.

In this Review, we aim to delineate potential avenues for future research that could lead to concrete interventions to improve adolescent mental health by considering mechanisms at the nexus between pre-existing processes known to increase adolescent mental health vulnerability and digital affordances introduced by social media. First, we describe the affordance approach to understanding the effects of social media. We then draw upon research on adolescent development, mental health and social media to describe behavioural, cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms by which social media use might amplify changes during adolescent development to increase mental health vulnerability during this period of life. The specific mechanisms within each category were chosen because they have a strong evidence base showing that they undergo substantive changes during adolescent development, are implicated in mental health risk and can be modulated by social media affordances. Although the ways in which social media can also improve mental health resilience are not the focus of our Review and therefore are not reviewed fully here, they are briefly discussed in relation to each mechanism. Finally, we discuss future research focused on how to systematically test the intersection between social media and adolescent mental health.

Social media affordances

To study the impact of social media on adolescent mental health, its diverse design elements and highly individualized uses must be conceptualized. Initial research predominately related access to or time spent on social media to mental health outcomes 46 , 69 , 70 . However, social media is not similar to a toxin or nutrient for which each exposure dose has a defined link to a health-related outcome (dose–response relationship) 56 . Social media is a diverse environment that cannot be summarized by the amount of time one spends interacting with it 71 , 72 , and individual experiences are highly varied 45 .

Previous psychological reviews often focused on social media ‘features’ 73 and ‘affordances’ 74 interchangeably. However, these terms have distinct definitions in communication science and information systems research. Social media features are components of the technology intentionally designed to enable users to perform specific actions, such as liking, reposting or uploading a story 75 , 76 . By contrast, affordances describe the perceptions of action possibilities users have when engaging with social media and its features, such as anonymity (the difficulty with which social media users can identify the source of a message) and quantifiability (how countable information is).

The term ‘affordance’ came from ecological psychology and visuomotor research, and was described as mainly determined by human perception 77 . ‘Affordance’ was later adopted for design and human–computer interaction contexts to refer to the action possibilities that are suggested to the user by the technology design 78 . Communication research synthesizes both views. Affordances are now typically understood as the perceived — and therefore flexible — action possibilities of digital environments, which are jointly shaped by the technology’s features and users’ idiosyncratic perceptions of those features 79 .

Latent action possibilities can vary across different users, uses and technologies 79 . For example, ‘stories’ are a feature of Instagram designed to share content between users. Stories can also be described in terms of affordances when users perceive them as a way to determine how long their content remains available on the platform (persistence) or who can see that content (visibility) 80 , 81 , 82 , 83 , 84 . Low persistence (also termed ephemerality) and comparatively low visibility can be achieved through a technology feature (Instagram stories), but are not an outcome of technology use itself; they are instead perceived action possibilities that can vary across different technologies, users and designs 79 .

The affordances approach is particularly valuable for theorizing at a level above individual social media apps or specific features, which makes this approach more resilient to technological changes or shifts in platform popularity 79 , 85 . However, the affordances approach can also be related back to specific types of social media by assessing the extent to which certain affordances are ‘built into’ a particular platform through feature design 35 . Furthermore, because affordances depend on individuals’ perceptions and actions, they are more aligned than features with a neurocognitive and behavioural perspective to social media use. Affordances, similar to neurocognitive and behavioural research, emphasize the role of the user (how the technology is perceived, interpreted and used) rather than technology design per se. In this sense, the affordances approach is essential to overcome technological determinism of mental health outcomes, which overly emphasizes the role of technology as the driver of outcomes but overlooks the agency and impact of the people in question 86 . This flexibility and alignment with psychological theory has contributed to the increasing popularity of the affordance approach 35 , 73 , 74 , 85 , 87 and previous reviews have explored relevant social media affordances in the context of interpersonal communication among adults and adolescents 35 , 88 , 89 , adolescent body image concerns 73 and work contexts 33 . Here, we focus on the affordances of social media that are relevant for adolescent development and its intersection with mental health (Table  1 ).

Behavioural mechanisms

Adolescents often use social media differently to adults, engaging with different platforms and features and, potentially, perceiving or making use of affordances in distinctive ways 35 . These usage differences might interact with developmental characteristics and changes to amplify mental health vulnerability (Fig.  3 ). We examine two behavioural mechanisms that might govern the impact of social media use on mental health: risky posting behaviours and self-presentation.

figure 3

Social media affordances can amplify the impact that common adolescent developmental mechanisms (behavioural, cognitive and neurobiological) have on mental health. At the behavioural level (top), affordances such as permanence and publicness lead to an increased impact of risk-taking behaviour on mental health compared with similar behaviours in non-mediated environments. At the cognitive level (middle), high quantifiability influences the effects of social comparison. At the neurobiological level (bottom), low synchronicity can amplify the effects of stress on the developing brain.

Risky posting behaviour

Sensation-seeking peaks in adolescence and self-regulation abilities are still not fully developed in this period of life 90 . Thus, adolescents often engage in more risky behaviours than other age groups 91 . Adolescents are more likely to take risks in situations involving peers 92 , 93 , perhaps because they are motivated to avoid social exclusion 94 , 95 . Whether adolescent risk-taking behaviour is inherently adaptive or maladaptive is debated. Although some risk-taking behaviours can be adaptive and part of typical development, others can increase mental health vulnerability. For example, data from a prospective UK panel study of more than 5,500 young people showed that engaging in more risky behaviours (including social and health risks) at age 16 years increases the odds of a range of adverse outcomes at age 18 years, such as depression, anxiety and substance abuse 96 .

Social media can increase adolescents’ engagement in risky behaviours both in non-mediated and mediated environments (environments in which the behaviour is executed in or through a technology, such as a mobile phone and social media). First, affordances such as quantifiability in conjunction with visibility and association (the degree with which links between people, between people and content or between a presenter and their audience can be articulated) can promote more risky behaviours in non-mediated environments and in-person social interactions. For example, posts from university students containing references to alcohol gain more likes than posts not referencing alcohol and liking such posts predicts an individual’s subsequent drinking habits 97 . Users expecting likes from their audience are incentivized to engage in riskier posting behaviour (such as more frequent or more extreme posts containing references to alcohol). The relationship between risky online behaviour and offline behaviour is supported by meta-analyses that found a positive correlation between adolescents’ social media use and their engagement in behaviours that might expose them to harm or risk of injury (for example, substance use or risky sexual behaviours) 98 . Further, affordances such as persistence and visibility can mean that risky behaviours in mediated and non-mediated environments remain public for long periods of time, potentially influencing how an adolescent is perceived by peers over the longer term 39 , 99 .

Adolescence can also be a time of more risky social media use. For most forms of semi-public and public social media use, users typically do not know who exactly will be able to see their posts. Thus, adolescents need to self-present to an ‘imagined audience’ 100 and avoid posting the wrong kind of content as the boundaries between different social spheres collapse (context collapse 101 ). However, young people can underestimate the risks of disclosing revealing information in a social media environment 102 . Affordances such as visibility, replicability (social media posts remain in the system and can be screenshotted and shared even if they are later deleted 39 ), association and persistence could heighten the risk of experiencing cyberbullying, victimization and online harassment 103 . For example, adolescents can forward privately received sexual images to larger friendship groups, increasing the risk of online harassment over the subject of the sexual images 104 . Further, low bandwidth (a relative lack of socio-emotional cues) and high anonymity have the potential to disinhibit interactions between users and make behaviours and reactions more extreme 105 , 106 . For example, anonymity was associated with more trolling behaviours during an online group discussion in an experiment with 242 undergraduate students 107 .

Thus, social media might drive more risky behaviours in both mediated and non-mediated contexts, increasing mental health vulnerability. However, the evidence is still not clear cut and often discounts adolescent agency and understanding. For example, mixed-methods research has shown that young people often understand the risks of posting private or sexual content and use social media apps that ensure that posts are deleted and inaccessible after short periods of time to counteract them 39 (even though posts can still be captured in the meantime). Future work will therefore need to investigate how adolescents understand and balance such risks and how such processes relate to social media’s impact on mental health.

Self-presentation and identity

The adolescent period is characterized by an abundance of self-presentation activities on social media 74 , where the drive to present oneself becomes a fundamental motivation for engagement 108 . These activities include disclosing, concealing and modifying one’s true self, and might involve deception, to convey a desired impression to an audience 109 . Compared with adults, adolescents more frequently take part in self-presentation 102 , which can encompass both realistic and idealized portrayals of themselves 110 . In adults, authentic self-presentation has been associated with increased well-being, and inauthentic presentation (such as when a person describes themselves in ways not aligned with their true self) has been associated with decreased well-being 111 , 112 , 113 .

Several social media affordances shape the self-presentation behaviours of adolescents. For example, the editability of social media profiles enables users to curate their online identity 84 , 114 . Editability is further enhanced by highly visible (public) self-presentations. Additionally, the constant availability of social media platforms enables adolescents to access and engage with their profiles at any time, and provides them with rapid quantitative feedback about their popularity among peers 89 , 115 . People receive more direct and public feedback on their self-presentation on social media than in other types of environment 116 , 117 . The affordances associated with self-presentation can have a particular impact during adolescence, a period characterized by identity development and exploration.

Social media environments might provide more opportunities than offline environments for shaping one’s identity. Indeed, public self-presentation has been found to invoke more prominent identity shifts (substantial changes in identity) compared with private self-presentation 118 , 119 . Concerns have been raised that higher Internet use is associated with decreased self-concept clarity. Only one study of 101 adolescents as well as adults reviewed in a 2021 meta-analysis 120 showed that the intensity of Facebook use (measured by the Facebook Intensity Scale) predicted a longitudinal decline in self-concept clarity 3 months later, but the converse was not the case and changes in self-concept clarity did not predict Facebook use 121 . This result is still not enough to show a causal relationship 121 . Further, the affordances of persistence and replicability could also curtail adolescents’ ability to explore their identity freely 122 .

By contrast, qualitative research has highlighted that social media enables adolescents to broaden their horizons, explore their identity and identify and reaffirm their values 123 . Social media can help self-presentation by enabling adolescents to elaborate on various aspects of their identity, such as ethnicity and race 124 or sexuality 125 . Social media affordances such as editability and visibility can also facilitate this process. Adolescents can modify and curate self-presentations online, try out new identities or express previously undisclosed aspects of their identity 126 , 127 . They can leverage social media affordances to present different facets of themselves to various social groups by using different profiles, platforms and self-censorship and curation of posts 128 , 129 . Presenting and exploring different aspects of one’s identity can have mental health implications for minority teens. Emerging research shows a positive correlation between well-being and problematic Internet use in transgender, non-binary and gender-diverse adolescents (age 13–18 years), and positive sentiment has been associated with online identity disclosures in transgender individuals with supportive networks (both adolescent and adult) 130 , 131 .

Cognitive mechanisms

Adolescents and adults might experience different socio-cognitive impacts from the same social media activity. In this section, we review four cognitive mechanisms via which social media and its affordances might influence the link between adolescent development and mental health vulnerabilities (Fig.  3 ). These mechanisms (self-concept development, social comparison, social feedback and exclusion) roughly align with a previous review that examined self-esteem and social media use 115 .

Self-concept development

Self-concept refers to a person’s beliefs and evaluations about their own qualities and traits 132 , which first develops and becomes more complex throughout childhood and then accelerates its development during adolescence 133 , 134 , 135 . Self-concept is shaped by socio-emotional processes such as self-appraisal and social feedback 134 . A negative and unstable self-concept has been associated with negative mental health outcomes 136 , 137 .

Perspective-taking abilities also develop during adolescence 133 , 138 , 139 , as does the processing of self-relevant stimuli (measured by self-referential memory tasks, which assess memory for self-referential trait adjectives 140 , 141 ). During adolescence, direct self-evaluations and reflected self-evaluations (how someone thinks others evaluate them) become more similar. Further, self-evaluations have a distinct positive bias during childhood, but this positivity bias decreases in adolescence as evaluations of the self are integrated with judgements of other people’s perspectives 142 . Indeed, negative self-evaluations peak in late adolescence (around age 19 years) 140 .

The impact of social media on the development of self-concept could be heightened during adolescence because of affordances such as personalization of content 143 (the degree to which content can be tailored to fit the identity, preferences or expectations of the receiver), which adapts the information young people are exposed to. Other affordances with similar impacts are quantifiability, availability (the accessibility of the technology as well as the user’s accessibility through the technology) and public visibility of interactions 89 , which render the evaluations of others more prominent and omnipresent. The prominence of social evaluation can pose long-term risks to mental health under certain conditions and for some users 144 , 145 . For example, receiving negative evaluations from others or being exposed to cyberbullying behaviours 146 , 147 can, potentially, have heightened impact at times of self-concept development.

A pioneering cross-sectional study of 150 adolescents showed that direct self-evaluations are more similar to reflected self-evaluations, and self-evaluations are more negative, in adolescents aged 11–21 years who estimate spending more time on social media 148 . Further, longitudinal data have shown bidirectional negative links between social media use and satisfaction with domains of the self (such as satisfaction with family, friends or schoolwork) 47 .

Although large-scale evidence is still unavailable, these findings raise the interesting prospect that social media might have a negative influence on perspective-taking and self-concept. There is less evidence for the potential positive influence of social media on these aspects of adolescent development, demonstrating an important research gap. Some researchers hypothesize that social media enables self-concept unification because it provides ample opportunity to find validation 89 . Research has also discussed how algorithmic curation of personalized social media feeds (for example, TikTok algorithms tailoring videos viewed to the user’s interests) enables users to reflect on their self-concept by being exposed to others’ experiences and perspectives 143 , an area where future research can provide important insights.

Social comparison

Social comparison (thinking about information about other people in relation to the self 149 ) also influences self-concept development and becomes particularly important during adolescence 133 , 150 . There are a range of social media affordances that can amplify the impact of social comparison on mental health. For example, quantifiability enables like or follower counts to be easily compared with others as a sign of status, which facilitates social ranking 151 , 152 , 153 , 154 . Studies of older adolescents and adults aged, on average, 20 years have also found that the number of likes or reactions received predict, in part, how successful users judge their self-presentation posts on Facebook 155 . Furthermore, personalization enables the content that users see on social media to be curated so as to be highly relevant and interesting for them, which should intensify comparisons. For example, an adolescent interested in sports and fitness content will receive personalized recommendations fitting those interests, which should increase the likelihood of comparisons with people portrayed in this content. In turn, the affordance of association can help adolescents surround themselves with similar peers and public personae online, enhancing social comparison effects 63 , 156 . Being able to edit posts (via the affordance of editability) has been argued to contribute to the positivity bias on social media: what is portrayed online is often more positive than the offline experience. Thus, upward comparisons are more likely to happen in online spaces than downward or lateral comparisons 157 . Lastly, the verifiability of others’ idealized self-presentations is often low, meaning that users have insufficient cues to gauge their authenticity 158 .

Engaging in comparisons on social media has been associated with depression in correlational studies 159 . Furthermore, qualitative research has shown that not receiving as many positive evaluations as expected (or if positive evaluations are not provided quickly enough) increases negative emotions in children and adolescents aged between age 9 and 19 years 39 . This result aligns with a reinforcement learning modelling study of Instagram data, which found that the likes a user receives on their own posts become less valuable and less predictive of future posting behaviour if others in their network receive more likes on their posts 160 . Although this study did not measure mood or mental health, it shows that the value of the likes are not static but inherently social; their impact depends on how many are typically received by other people in the same network.

Among the different types of social comparison that adolescents engage in (comparing one’s achievements, social status or lifestyle), the most substantial concerns have been raised about body-related comparisons. One review suggested that social media affordances create a ‘perfect storm’ for body image concerns that can contribute to both socio-emotional and eating disorders 73 . Social media affordances might increase young people’s focus on other people’s appearances as well as on their own appearance by showing idealized, highly edited images, providing quantified feedback and making the ability to associate and compare oneself with peers constantly available 161 , 162 . The latter puts adolescents who are less popular or receive less social support at particular risk of low self-image and social distress 35 .

Affordances enable more prominent and explicit social comparisons in social media environments relative to offline environments 158 , 159 , 163 , 164 , 165 . However, this association could have a positive impact on mental health 164 , 166 . Initial evidence suggests beneficial outcomes of upward comparisons on social media, which can motivate behaviour change and yield positive downstream effects on mental health 164 , 166 . Positive motivational effects (inspiration) have been observed among young adults for topics such as travelling and exploring nature, as well as fitness and other health behaviours, which can all improve mental health 167 . Importantly, inspiration experiences are not a niche phenomenon on social media: an experience sampling study of 353 Dutch adolescents (mean age 13–15 years) found that participants reported some level of social media-induced inspiration in 33% of the times they were asked to report on this over the course of 3 weeks 168 . Several experimental and longitudinal studies show that inspiration is linked to upward comparison on social media 157 , 164 , 166 . However, the positive, motivating side of social comparison on social media has only been examined in a few studies and requires additional investigation.

Social feedback

Adolescence is also a period of social reorientation, when peers tend to become more important than family 169 , peer acceptance becomes increasingly relevant 170 , 171 , 172 and young people spend increasing amounts of time with peers 173 . In parallel, there is a heightened sensitivity to negative socio-emotional or self-referential cues 140 , 174 , higher expectation of being rejected by others 175 and internalization of such rejection 142 , 176 compared with other phases in life development. A meta-analysis of both adolescents and adults found that oversensitivity to social rejection is moderately associated with both depression and anxiety 177 .

Social media affordances might amplify the potential impact of social feedback on mental health. Wanting to be accepted by peers and increased susceptibility to social rewards could be a motivator for using social media in the first place 178 . Indeed, receiving likes as social reward activated areas of the brain (such as the nucleus accumbens) that are also activated by monetary reward 179 . Quantifiability amplifies peer acceptance and rejection (via like counts), and social rejection has been linked to adverse mental health outcomes 170 , 180 , 181 , 182 . Social media can also increase feelings of being evaluated, the risk of social rejection and rumination about potential rejection due to affordances such as quantifiability, synchronicity (the degree to which an interaction happens in real time) and variability of social rewards (the degree to which social interaction and feedback occur on variable time schedules). For example, one study of undergraduate students found that active communication such as messaging was associated with feeling better after Facebook use; however, this was not the case if the communication led to negative feelings such as rumination (for example, after no responses to the messages) 183 .

In a study assessing threatened social evaluation online 184 , participants were asked to record a statement about themselves and were told their statements would be rated by others. To increase the authenticity of the threat, participants were asked to rate other people’s recordings. Threatened social evaluation online in this study decreased mood, most prominently in people with high sensitivity to social rejection. Adolescents who are more sensitive to social rejection report more severe depressive symptoms and maladaptive ruminative brooding in both mediated and non-mediated social environments, and this association is most prominent in early adolescence 185 . Not receiving as much online social approval as peers led to more severe depressive symptoms in a study of American ninth-grade adolescents (between age 14 and 15 years), especially those who were already experiencing peer victimization 153 . Furthermore, individuals with lower self-esteem post more negative and less positive content than individuals with higher self-esteem. Posted negative content receives less social reward and recognition from others than positive content, possibly creating a vicious cycle 186 . Negative experiences pertaining to social exclusion and status are also risk factors for socio-emotional disorders 180 .

The impact of social media experiences on self-esteem can be very heterogeneous, varying substantially across individuals. As a benefit, positive social feedback obtained via social media can increase users’ self-esteem 115 , an association also found among adolescents 187 . For instance, receiving likes on one’s profile or posted photographs can bolster self-esteem in the short term 144 , 188 . A study linking behavioural data and self-reports from Facebook users found that receiving quick responses on public posts increased a sense of social support and decreased loneliness 189 . Furthermore, a review of reviews consistently documented that users who report more social media use also perceive themselves to have more social resources and support online 52 , although this association has mostly been studied among young adults using social network sites such as Facebook. Whether such social feedback benefits extend to adolescents’ use of platforms centred on content consumption (such as TikTok or Instagram) is an open question.

Social inclusion and exclusion

Adolescents are more sensitive to the negative emotional impacts of being excluded than are adults 170 , 190 . It has been proposed that, as the importance of social affiliation increases during this period of life 134 , 191 , 192 , adolescents are more sensitive to a range of social stimuli, regardless of valence 193 . These include social feedback (such as compliments or likes) 95 , 194 , negative socio-emotional cues (such as negative facial expressions or social exclusion) 174 and social rejection 172 , 185 . By contrast, social inclusion (via friendships in adolescence) is protective against emotional disorders 195 and more social support is related to higher adolescent well-being 196 .

Experiencing ostracism and exclusion online decreases self-esteem and positive emotion 197 . This association has been found in vignette experiments where participants received no, only a few or a lot of likes 198 , or experiments that used mock-ups of social media sites where others received more likes than participants 153 . Being ostracized (not receiving attention or feedback) or rejected through social media features (receiving dislikes and no likes) is also associated with a reduced sense of belonging, meaningfulness, self-esteem and control 199 . Similar results were found when ostracism was experienced over messaging apps, such as not receiving a reply via WhatsApp 200 .

Evidence on whether social media also enables adolescents to experience positive social inclusion is mostly indirect and mixed. Some longitudinal surveys have found that prosocial feedback received on social media during major life events (such as university admissions) helps to buffer against stress 201 . Adult participants of a longitudinal study reported that social media offered more informational support than offline contexts, but offline contexts more often offered emotional or instrumental support 202 . Higher social network site use is, on average, associated with a perception of having more social resources and support in adults (for an overview of meta-analyses, see ref. 52 ). However, most of these studies have not investigated social support among adolescents, and it is unclear whether early findings (for example, on Facebook or Twitter) generalize to a social media landscape more strongly characterized by content consumption than social interaction (such as Instagram or TikTok).

Still, a review of social media use and offline interpersonal outcomes among adolescents documents both positive (sense of belonging and social capital) and negative (alienation from peers and perceived isolation) correlates 203 . Experience sampling research on emotional support among young adults has further shown that online social support is received and perceived as effective, and its perceived effectiveness is similar to in-person social support 204 . Social media use also has complex associations with friendship closeness among adolescents. For example, one experience sampling study found that greater use of WhatsApp or Instagram is associated with higher friendship closeness among adolescents; however, within-person examinations over time showed small negative associations 205 .

Neurobiological mechanisms

The long-term impact of environmental changes such as social media use on mental health might be amplified because adolescence is a period of considerable neurobiological development 95 (Fig.  3 ). During adolescence, overall cortical grey matter declines and white matter increases 206 , 207 . Development is particularly protracted in brain regions associated with social cognition and executive functions such as planning, decision-making and inhibiting prepotent responses. The changes in grey and white matter are thought to reflect axonal growth, myelination and synaptic reorganization, which are mechanisms of neuroplasticity influenced by the environment 208 . For example, research in rodents has demonstrated that adolescence is a sensitive period for social input, and that social isolation in adolescence has unique and more deleterious consequences for neural, behavioural and mental health development than social isolation before puberty or in adulthood 206 , 209 . There is evidence that brain regions involved in motivation and reward show greater activation to rewarding and motivational stimuli (such as appetitive stimuli and the presence of peers) in early and/or mid adolescence compared with other age groups 210 , 211 , 212 , 213 , 214 .

Little is known about the potential links between social media and neurodevelopment due to the paucity of research investigating these associations. Furthermore, causal chains (for example, social media increasing stress, which in turn influences the brain) have not yet been accurately delineated. However, it would be amiss not to recognize that brain development during adolescence forms part of the biological basis of mental health vulnerability and should therefore be considered. Indeed, the brain is proposed to be particularly plastic in adolescence and susceptible to environmental stimuli, both positive and negative 208 . Thus, even if adults and adolescents experienced the same affective consequences from social media use (such as increases in peer comparison or stress), these consequences might have a greater impact in adolescence.

A cross-sectional study (with some longitudinal elements) suggested that habitual checking of social media (for example, checking for rewards such as likes) might exacerbate reward sensitivity processes, leading to long-term hypersensitization of the reward system 215 . Specifically, frequently checking social media was associated with reduced activation in brain regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in response to anticipated social feedback in young people. Brain activation during the same social feedback task was measured over subsequent years. Upon follow-up, anticipating feedback was associated with increased activation of the same brain regions among the individuals who checked social media frequently initially 215 . Although longitudinal brain imaging measurements enabled trajectories of brain development to be specified, the measures of social media use were only acquired once in the first wave of data collection. The study therefore cannot account for confounds such as personality traits, which might influence both social media checking behaviours and brain development. Other studies of digital screen use and brain development have found no impact on adolescent functional brain organization 216 .

Brain development and heightened neuroplasticity 208 render adolescence a particularly sensitive period with potentially long-term impacts into adulthood. It is possible that social media affordances that underpin increased checking and reward-seeking behaviours (such as quantifiability, variability of social rewards and permanent availability of peers) might have long-term consequences on reward processing when experienced during adolescence. However, this suggestion is still speculative and not backed up by evidence 217 .

Stress is another example of the potential amplifying effect of social media on adolescent mental health vulnerability due to neural development. Adolescents show higher stress reactivity because of maturational changes to, and increased reactivity in, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis 218 , 219 . Compared with children and adults, adolescents experience an increase in self-consciousness and associated emotional states such as self-reported embarrassment and related physiological measures of arousal (such as skin conductance), and heightened neural response patterns compared with adults, when being evaluated or observed by peers 220 . Similarly, adolescents (age 13–17 years) show higher stress responses (higher levels of cortisol or blood pressure) compared with children (age 7–12 years) when they perform in front of others or experience social rejection 221 .

Such changes in adolescence might confer heightened risk for the onset of mental health conditions, especially socio-emotional disorders 6 . Both adolescent rodents and humans show prolonged hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal activation after experiencing stress compared with conspecifics of different ages 218 , 219 . In animal models, stress during adolescence has been shown to result in increased anxiety levels in adulthood 222 and alterations in emotional and cognitive development 223 . Furthermore, human studies have linked stress in adolescence to a higher risk of mental health disorder onset 218 and reviews of cross-species work have illustrated a range of brain changes due to adolescent stress 224 , 225 .

There is still little conclusive neurobiological evidence about social media use and stress, and a lack of understanding about which affordances might be involved (although there has been a range of work studying digital stress; Box  1 ). Studies of changes in cortisol levels or hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal functioning and their relation to social media use have been mixed and inconclusive 226 , 227 . These results could be due to the challenge of studying stress responses in adolescents, particularly as cortisol fluctuates across the day and one-point readings can be unreliable. However, the increased stress sensitivity during the adolescent developmental period might mean that social media use can have a long-term influence on mental health due to neurobiological mechanisms. These processes are therefore important to understand in future research.

Box 1 Digital stress

Digital stress is not a unified construct. Thematic content analyses have categorized digital stress into type I stressors (for example, mean attacks, cyberbullying or shaming) and type II stressors (for example, interpersonal stress due to pressure to stay available) 260 . Other reviews have noted its complexity, and categorized digital stress into availability stress (stress that results from having to be constantly available), approval anxiety (anxiety regarding others’ reaction to their own profile, posts or activities online), fear of missing out (stress about being absent from or not experiencing others’ rewarding experiences) and communication overload (stress due to the scale, intensity and frequency of online communication) 261 .

Digital stress has been systematically linked to negative mental health outcomes. Higher digital stress was longitudinally associated with higher depressive symptoms in a questionnaire study 262 . Higher social media stress was also longitudinally related to poorer sleep outcomes in girls (but not boys) 263 . Studies and reviews have linked cyberbullying victimization (a highly stressful experience) to decreased mental health outcomes such as depression, and psychosocial outcomes such as self-esteem 103 , 146 , 147 , 264 , 265 . A systematic review of both adolescents and adults found a medium association ( r  = 0.26–0.34) between different components of digital stress and psychological distress outcomes such as anxiety, depression or loneliness, which was not moderated by age or sex (except for connection overload) 266 . However, the causal structure giving rise to such results is still far from clear. For example, surveys have linked higher stress levels to more problematic social media use and fear of missing out 267 , 268 .

Thus, the impact of digital stress on mental health is probably complex and influenced by the type of digital stressor and various affordances. For example, visibility and availability increase fear of negative public evaluation 269 and high availability and a social norm of responding quickly to messages drive constant monitoring in adolescents due to a persistent fear of upsetting friends 270 .

A range of relevant evidence from qualitative and quantitative studies documents that adolescents often ruminate about online interactions and messages. For example, online salience (constantly thinking about communication, content or events happening online) was positively associated with stress on both between-person and within-person levels in a cross-sectional quota sample of adults and three diary studies of young adults 271 , 272 . Online salience has also been associated with lower well-being in a pre-registered study of momentary self-reports from young adults with logged online behaviours. However, this study also noted that positive thoughts were related to higher well-being 273 . Furthermore, although some studies found no associations between the amount of communication and digital stress 272 , a cross-sectional study found that younger users’ (age 14–34 years and 35–49 years) perception of social pressure to be constantly available was related to communication load (measured by questions about the amount of use, as well as the urge to check email and social media) and Internet multitasking, whereas this was not the case for older users aged 50–85 years 274 . By contrast, communication load and perceived stress were associated only among older users.

Summary and future directions

To help to understand the potential role of social media in the decline of adolescent mental health over the past decade, researchers should study the mechanisms linking social media, adolescent development and mental health. Specifically, social media environments might amplify the socio-cognitive processes that render adolescents more vulnerable to mental health conditions in the first place. We outline various mechanisms at three levels of adolescent development — behavioural, cognitive and neurobiological — that could be involved in the decline of adolescent mental health as a function of social media engagement. To do so, we delineate specific social media affordances, such as quantification of social feedback or anonymity, which can also have positive impacts on mental health.

Our Review sets out clear recommendations for future research on the intersection of social media and adolescent mental health. The foundation of this research lies in the existing literature investigating the underlying processes that heighten adolescents’ risk of developing socio-emotional disorders. Zooming in on the potential mechanistic targets impacted by social media uses and affordances will produce specific research questions to facilitate controlled and systematic scientific inquiry relevant for intervention and translation. This approach encourages researchers to pinpoint the mechanisms and levels of explanation they want to include and will enable them to identify what factors to additionally consider, such as participants’ age 60 , the specific mental health outcomes being measured, the types of social media being examined and the populations under study 52 , 228 . Targeted and effective research should prioritize the most promising areas of study and acknowledge that all research approaches have inherent limitations 229 . Researchers must embrace methodological diversity, which in turn will facilitate triangulation. Surveys, experience sampling designs in conjunction with digital trace data, as well as experimental or neuroimaging paradigms and computational modelling (such as reinforcement learning) can all be used to address research questions comprehensively 230 . Employing such a multi-method approach enables the convergence of evidence and strengthens the reliability of findings 231 .

Mental health and developmental research can also become more applicable to the study of social media by considering how studies might already be exploring features of the digital environment, such as its design features and perceived affordances. Many cognitive neuroscience studies that investigate social processes and mental health during adolescence necessarily design tasks that can be completed in controlled experimental or brain scanning environments. Consequently, they tend to focus on digitally mediated interactions. However, researchers conceptualize and generalize their results to face-to-face interactions. For example, it is common across the discipline to not explicitly describe the interactions under study as being about social processes in digital environments (such as studies that assess social feedback based on the number of ‘thumbs up’ or ‘thumbs down’ received in social media 232 ). Considering whether cognitive neuroscience studies include key affordances of mediated (or non-mediated) environments, and discussing these in published papers, will make studies searchable within the field of social media research, enabling researchers to broaden the impact of their work and systematically specify generalizations to offline environments 233 .

To bridge the gap between knowledge about mediated and non-mediated social environments, it is essential to directly compare the two 233 . It is often assumed that negative experiences online have a detrimental impact on mental health. However, it remains unclear whether this mechanism is present in both mediated and non-mediated spaces or whether it is specific to the mediated context. For instance, our Review highlights that the quantification of social feedback through likes is an important affordance of social media 160 . Feedback on social media platforms might therefore elicit a greater sense of certainty because it is quantified compared with the more subjective and open-to-interpretation feedback received face to face 151 . Conducting experiments in which participants receive feedback that is more or less quantified and uncertain, specifically designed to compare mediated and non-mediated environments, would provide valuable insights. Such research efforts could also establish connections with computational neuroscience studies demonstrating that people tend to learn faster from stimuli that are less uncertain 234 .

We have chosen not to make recommendations concerning interventions targeting social media use to improve adolescent mental health for several reasons. First, we did not fully consider the bidirectional interactions between environment and development 35 , 235 , or the factors modulating adolescents’ differential susceptibility to the effects of social media 45 , 58 . For example, mental health status also influences how social media is used 47 , 58 , 59 , 236 , 237 (Box  2 ). These bidirectional interactions could be addressed using network or complexity science approaches 238 . Second, we do not yet know how the potential mechanisms by which social media might increase mental health vulnerability compare in magnitude, importance, scale and ease and/or cost of intervention with other factors and mechanisms that are already well known to influence mental health, such as poverty or loneliness. Last, social media use will probably interact with these predictors in ways that have not been delineated and can also support mental health resilience (for example, through social support or online self-help programmes). These complexities should be considered in future research, which will need to pinpoint not just the existence of mechanisms but their relative importance, to identify policy and intervention priorities.

Our Review has used a broad definition of mental health. Focusing on specific diagnostic or transdiagnostic symptomatology might reveal different mechanisms of interest. Furthermore, our Review is limited to mechanisms related to behaviour and neurocognitive development, disregarding other levels of explanation (such as genetics and culture) 34 , and also studying predominately Western-centric samples 239 . Mechanisms do not operate solely in linear pathways but exist within networks of interacting risk and resilience factors, characterized by non-linear and complex dynamics across diverse timescales 9 . Mechanisms and predisposing factors can interact and combine, amplifying mental health vulnerability. Mental health can be considered a dynamic system in which gradual changes to external conditions can have substantial downstream consequences due to system properties such as feedback loops 240 , 241 , 242 . These consequences are especially prominent in times of change and pre-existing vulnerability, such as adolescence 10 .

Indeed, if social media is a contributing factor to the current decline in adolescent mental health, as is commonly assumed, then it is important to identify and investigate mechanisms that are specifically tailored to the adolescent age range and make the case for why they matter. Without a thorough examination of these mechanisms and policy analysis to indicate whether they should be a priority to address, there is insufficient evidence to support the hypothesis that social media is the primary — or even just an influential and important — driver of mental health declines. Researchers need to stop studying social media as monolithic and uniform, and instead study its features, affordances and outcomes by leveraging a range of methods including experiments, questionnaires, qualitative research and industry data. Ultimately, this comprehensive approach will enhance researchers’ ability to address the potential challenges that the digital era poses on adolescent mental health.

Box 2 Effects of mental health on social media use

Although a lot of scientific discussion has focused on the impact of social media use on mental health, cross-sectional studies cannot differentiate between whether social media use is influencing mental health or mental health is influencing social media use, or a third factor is influencing both 51 . It is likely that mental health status influences social media use creating reinforcing cycles of behaviour, something that has been considered in the communication sciences literature under the term ‘transactional media effects’ 58 , 236 , 237 . According to communication science models, media use and its consequences are components of reciprocal processes 275 .

There are similar models in mental health research. For example, people’s moods influence their judgements of events, which can lead to self-perpetuating cycles of negativity (or positivity); a mechanism called ‘mood congruency’ 276 . Behavioural studies have also shown that people experiencing poor mental health behave in ways that decrease their opportunity to experience environmental reward such as social activities, maintaining poor mental health 277 , 278 . Although for many people these behaviours are a form of coping (for example, by avoiding stressful circumstances), they often worsen symptoms of mental health conditions 279 .

Some longitudinal studies found that a decrease in adolescent well-being predicted an increase in social media use 1 year later 47 , 59 . However, other studies have found no relationships between well-being and social media use over long-term or daily time windows 45 , 46 . One reason behind the heterogeneity of the results could be that how mental health impacts social media use is highly individual 45 , 280 .

Knowledge on the impact of mental health on social media use is still in its infancy and studies struggle to reach coherent conclusions. However, findings from the mental health literature can be used to generate hypotheses about how aspects of mental health might impact social media use. For example, it has been repeatedly found that young people with anxiety or eating disorders engage in more social comparisons than individuals without these disorders 281 , 282 , and adolescents with depression report more unfavourable social comparisons on social media than adolescents without depression 283 . Similar results have been found for social feedback seeking (for example, reassurance), including in social media environments 159 . Specifically, depressive symptoms were more associated with social comparison and feedback seeking, and these associations were stronger in women and in adolescents who were less popular. Individuals from the general population with lower self-esteem post more negative and less positive content than individuals with higher self-esteem, which in turn is associated with receiving less positive feedback from others 185 . There are therefore a wide range of possible ways in which diverse aspects of mental health might influence specific facets of how social media is used — and, in turn, how it ends up impacting the user.

Savin-Williams, R. Adolescence: An Ethological Perspective (Springer, 1987).

Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D. & Patton, G. C. The age of adolescence. Lancet Child. Adolesc. Health 2 , 223–228 (2018).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Paus, T., Keshavan, M. & Giedd, J. N. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 , 947–957 (2008).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Solmi, M. et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol. Psychiatry 27 , 281–295 (2022).

Orben, A., Lucas, R. E., Fuhrmann, D. & Kievit, R. A. Trajectories of adolescent life satisfaction. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 9 , 211808 (2022).

Rapee, R. M. et al. Adolescent development and risk for the onset of social-emotional disorders: a review and conceptual model. Behav. Res. Ther. 123 , 103501 (2019). This review describes why adolescence is a sensitive period for mental health vulnerability.

Arango, C. et al. Risk and protective factors for mental disorders beyond genetics: an evidence‐based atlas. World Psychiatry 20 , 417–436 (2021).

Ioannidis, K., Askelund, A. D., Kievit, R. A. & van Harmelen, A.-L. The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Med. 18 , 32 (2020).

Kraemer, H. C., Stice, E., Kazdin, A., Offord, D. & Kupfer, D. How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. AJP 158 , 848–856 (2001).

Article   Google Scholar  

Hankin, B. L. & Abramson, L. Y. Development of gender differences in depression: an elaborated cognitive vulnerability–transactional stress theory. Psychol. Bull. 127 , 773–796 (2001).

Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., Natarajan, L. & Pickles, A. Trends in adolescent emotional problems in England: a comparison of two national cohorts twenty years apart: twenty-year trends in emotional problems. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 51 , 885–894 (2010).

Pitchforth, J. M., Viner, R. M. & Hargreaves, D. S. Trends in mental health and wellbeing among children and young people in the UK: a repeated cross-sectional study, 2000–14. Lancet 388 , S93 (2016).

Coley, R. L., O’Brien, M. & Spielvogel, B. Secular trends in adolescent depressive symptoms: growing disparities between advantaged and disadvantaged schools. J. Youth Adolescence 48 , 2087–2098 (2019).

Patalay, P. & Gage, S. H. Changes in millennial adolescent mental health and health-related behaviours over 10 years: a population cohort comparison study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 48 , 1650–1664 (2019).

Pitchforth, J. M. et al. Mental health and well-being trends among children and young people in the UK, 1995–2014: analysis of repeated cross-sectional national health surveys. Psychol. Med. 49 , 1275–1285 (2019).

Plana‐Ripoll, O. et al. Temporal changes in sex‐ and age‐specific incidence profiles of mental disorders—a nationwide study from 1970 to 2016. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 145 , 604–614 (2022).

Twenge, J. M., Cooper, A. B., Joiner, T. E., Duffy, M. E. & Binau, S. G. Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005–2017. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128 , 185–199 (2019).

van Vuuren, C. L., Uitenbroek, D. G., van der Wal, M. F. & Chinapaw, M. J. M. Sociodemographic differences in 10-year time trends of emotional and behavioural problems among adolescents attending secondary schools in Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 27 , 1621–1631 (2018).

Collishaw, S. Annual research review: secular trends in child and adolescent mental health. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 56 , 370–393 (2015).

Goodwin, R. D. et al. Trends in U.S. depression prevalence from 2015 to 2020: the widening treatment gap. Am. J. Prev. Med. 63 , 726–733 (2022).

Mojtabai, R. & Olfson, M. National trends in mental health care for US adolescents. JAMA Psychiatry 77 , 703 (2020).

Mojtabai, R., Olfson, M. & Han, B. National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults. Pediatrics 138 , e20161878 (2016).

Goodwin, R. D., Weinberger, A. H., Kim, J. H., Wu, M. & Galea, S. Trends in anxiety among adults in the United States, 2008–2018: rapid increases among young adults. J. Psychiatr. Res. 130 , 441–446 (2020).

Beerten, S. G. et al. Trends in the registration of anxiety in Belgian primary care from 2000 to 2021: a registry-based study. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 73 , e460–e467 (2022).

Walrave, R. et al. Trends in the epidemiology of depression and comorbidities from 2000 to 2019 in Belgium. BMC Prim. Care 23 , 163 (2022).

Vuorre, M. & Przybylski, A. K. Global well-being and mental health in the internet age. Clin. Psychol. Sci . https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026231207791 (2023).

Steffen, A., Thom, J., Jacobi, F., Holstiege, J. & Bätzing, J. Trends in prevalence of depression in Germany between 2009 and 2017 based on nationwide ambulatory claims data. J. Affect. Disord. 271 , 239–247 (2020).

Ford, T. Editorial Perspective: why I am now convinced that emotional disorders are increasingly common among young people in many countries. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr. 61 , 1275–1277 (2020).

McElroy, E., Tibber, M., Fearon, P., Patalay, P. & Ploubidis, G. B. Socioeconomic and sex inequalities in parent‐reported adolescent mental ill‐health: time trends in four British birth cohorts. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 64 , 758–767 (2022).

OECD. Society at a Glance 2019: OECD Social Indicators (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2019).

Ofcom. Online Nation (2021). Ofcom.org.uk https://www.ofcom.org.uk/research-and-data/online-research/online-nation (2022).

Anderson, M. & Jiang, J. Teens’ Social Media Habits and Experiences (Pew Research Center, 2018).

McFarland, L. A. & Ployhart, R. E. Social media: a contextual framework to guide research and practice. J. Appl. Psychol. 100 , 1653–1677 (2015).

Büchi, M. Digital well-being theory and research. N. Media Soc. 26 , 172–189 (2024).

Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S. & Prinstein, M. J. Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: part 1—a theoretical framework and application to dyadic peer relationships. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 21 , 267–294 (2018). This landmark paper applies the idea of affordances to understanding the impact of social media on adolescent social relationships.

Taffel, S. Perspectives on the postdigital: beyond rhetorics of progress and novelty. Convergence 22 , 324–338 (2016).

Papacharissi, Z. We have always been social. Soc. Media + Society 1 , 205630511558118 (2015).

Google Scholar  

Crone, E. A. & Konijn, E. A. Media use and brain development during adolescence. Nat. Commun. 9 , 1–10 (2018). This article describes adolescent cognitive and neural development and its intersection with new types of technology.

Weinstein, E. & James, C. Behind Their Screens: What Teens Are Facing (and Adults Are Missing) (MIT Press, 2022).

Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L. & Martin, G. N. Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates among U.S. adolescents after 2010 and links to increased new media screen time. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 6 , 3–17 (2017).

Gunnell, D., Kidger, J. & Elvidge, H. Adolescent mental health in crisis. BMJ 361 , k2608 (2018).

Odgers, C. L., Schueller, S. M. & Ito, M. Screen time, social media use, and adolescent development. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 2 , 485–502 (2020).

Valkenburg, P. M., Meier, A. & Beyens, I. Social media use and its impact on adolescent mental health: an umbrella review of the evidence. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44 , 58–68 (2022).

Kreski, N. et al. Social media use and depressive symptoms among United States adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 68 , 572–579 (2020).

Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L. & Valkenburg, P. M. The effect of social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. Sci. Rep. 10 , 10763 (2020). This landmark paper highlights that the impacts of social media on well-being are highly individual.

Jensen, M., George, M. J., Russell, M. R. & Odgers, C. L. Young adolescents’ digital technology use and mental health symptoms: little evidence of longitudinal or daily linkages. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7 , 1416–1433 (2019).

Orben, A., Dienlin, T. & Przybylski, A. K. Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116 , 10226–10228 (2019).

Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S. & Gentzkow, M. The welfare effects of social media. Am. Economic Rev. 110 , 629–676 (2020).

Nassen, L.-M., Vandebosch, H., Poels, K. & Karsay, K. Opt-out, abstain, unplug. A systematic review of the voluntary digital disconnection literature. Telemat. Inform. 81 , 101980 (2023).

Dienlin, T. & Johannes, N. The impact of digital technology use on adolescent well-being. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 22 , 135–142 (2020).

Odgers, C. L. & Jensen, M. R. Annual research review: adolescent mental health in the digital age: facts, fears, and future directions. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 61 , 336–348 (2020).

Meier, A. & Reinecke, L. Computer-mediated communication, social media, and mental health: a conceptual and empirical meta-review. Commun. Res. 48 , 1182–1209 (2021). This review provides a hierarchical taxonomy of the levels of analysis at which social media can be conceptualized and measured.

Orben, A. Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 55 , 407–414 (2020).

Bell, V., Bishop, D. V. M. & Przybylski, A. K. The debate over digital technology and young people. BMJ 351 , h3064 (2015).

Online Safety Act 2023. legislation.gov.uk , https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/2023/50/enacted (2023).

Hawkes, N. CMO report is unable to shed light on impact of screen time and social media on children’s health. BMJ 364 , l643 (2019).

US Department of Health and Human Services. Social Media and Youth Mental Health: The U.S. Surgeon General’s Advisory (2023).

Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. The differential susceptibility to media effects model: differential susceptibility to media effects model. J. Commun. 63 , 221–243 (2013). This landmark paper examines how the impact of media is influenced by individual differences.

Orben, A., Przybylski, A. K., Blakemore, S.-J. & Kievit, R. A. Windows of developmental sensitivity to social media. Nat. Commun. 13 , 1649 (2022). This large-scale data analysis shows that adolescent development potentially influences how social media impacts well-being.

Orben, A. & Blakemore, S.-J. How social media affects teen mental health: a missing link. Nature 614 , 410–412 (2023).

Shaw, H. et al. Quantifying smartphone “use”: choice of measurement impacts relationships between “usage” and health. Technol. Mind Behav . 1 , https://doi.org/10.1037/tmb0000022 (2020).

Parry, D. A. et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of discrepancies between logged and self-reported digital media use. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5 , 1535–1547 (2021).

Verduyn, P., Gugushvili, N. & Kross, E. Do social networking sites influence well-being? The extended active-passive model. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 31 , 62–68 (2022).

Davidson, B. I., Shaw, H. & Ellis, D. A. Fuzzy constructs in technology usage scales. Comput. Hum. Behav. 133 , 107206 (2022).

Shaw, D. J., Kaye, L. K., Ngombe, N., Kessler, K. & Pennington, C. R. It’s not what you do, it’s the way that you do it: an experimental task delineates among passive, reactive and interactive styles of behaviour on social networking sites. PLoS ONE 17 , e0276765 (2022).

Griffioen, N., Van Rooij, M., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A. & Granic, I. Toward improved methods in social media research. Technol. Mind Behav . 1 , https://doi.org/10.1037/tmb0000005 (2020).

Valkenburg, P. M. Social media use and well-being: what we know and what we need to know. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 45 , 101294 (2022).

Yang, C., Holden, S. M. & Ariati, J. Social media and psychological well-being among youth: the multidimensional model of social media use. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 24 , 631–650 (2021).

Kelly, Y., Zilanawala, A., Booker, C. & Sacker, A. Social media use and adolescent mental health: findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. EClinicalMedicine 6 , 59–68 (2019).

Orben, A. & Przybylski, A. K. The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3 , 173–182 (2019).

Sultan, M., Scholz, C. & van den Bos, W. Leaving traces behind: using social media digital trace data to study adolescent wellbeing. Comput. Hum. Behav. Rep. 10 , 100281 (2023).

Kaye, L., Orben, A., Ellis, D., Hunter, S. & Houghton, S. The conceptual and methodological mayhem of “screen time”. IJERPH 17 , 3661 (2020).

Choukas-Bradley, S., Roberts, S. R., Maheux, A. J. & Nesi, J. The perfect storm: a developmental–sociocultural framework for the role of social media in adolescent girls’ body image concerns and mental health. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 25 , 681–701 (2022). This review focuses on how social media can influence adolescent development of body image.

Moreno, M. A. & Uhls, Y. T. Applying an affordances approach and a developmental lens to approach adolescent social media use. Digital Health 5 , 205520761982667 (2019).

Smock, A. D., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C. & Wohn, D. Y. Facebook as a toolkit: a uses and gratification approach to unbundling feature use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27 , 2322–2329 (2011).

Bayer, J. B., Triêu, P. & Ellison, N. B. Social media elements, ecologies, and effects. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71 , 471–497 (2020).

Gibson, J. J. The Scological Approach to Visual Perception (Houghton Mifflin, 1979).

Norman, D. A. The Psychology of Everyday Things (Basic Books, 1988).

Evans, S. K., Pearce, K. E., Vitak, J. & Treem, J. W. Explicating affordances: a conceptual framework for understanding affordances in communication research. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 22 , 35–52 (2017).

Bayer, J. B., Ellison, N. B., Schoenebeck, S. Y. & Falk, E. B. Sharing the small moments: ephemeral social interaction on Snapchat. Information . Commun. Soc. 19 , 956–977 (2016).

Fox, J. & McEwan, B. Distinguishing technologies for social interaction: the perceived social affordances of communication channels scale. Commun. Monogr. 84 , 298–318 (2017).

Kreling, R., Meier, A. & Reinecke, L. Feeling authentic on social media: subjective authenticity across instagram stories and posts. Soc. Media + Society 8 , 205630512210862 (2022).

Leonardi, P. M. Social media, knowledge sharing, and innovation: toward a theory of communication visibility. Inf. Syst. Res. 25 , 796–816 (2014).

Treem, J. W. & Leonardi, P. M. Social media use in organizations: exploring the affordances of visibility, editability, persistence, and association. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 36 , 143–189 (2013).

Ellison, N. B., Pyle, C. & Vitak, J. Scholarship on well-being and social media: a sociotechnical perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 46 , 101340 (2022).

Orben, A. The Sisyphean cycle of technology panics. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15 , 1143–1157 (2020).

Granic, I., Morita, H. & Scholten, H. Beyond screen time: identity development in the digital age. Psychol. Inq. 31 , 195–223 (2020). This perspective discusses how adolescent identity development might be impacted by digital platforms including social media and video games.

Lieberman, A. & Schroeder, J. Two social lives: how differences between online and offline interaction influence social outcomes. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 31 , 16–21 (2020).

Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. Online communication among adolescents: an integrated model of its attraction, opportunities, and risks. J. Adolesc. Health 48 , 121–127 (2011).

Steinberg, L. et al. Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. Dev. Sci. 21 , e12532 (2018).

Blakemore, S.-J. & Robbins, T. W. Decision-making in the adolescent brain. Nat. Neurosci. 15 , 1184–1191 (2012).

Steinberg, L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Rev. 28 , 78–106 (2008).

Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K. & Steinberg, L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry: peer influence on risk taking. Dev. Sci. 14 , F1–F10 (2011).

Blakemore, S.-J. Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 116–122 (2018).

Blakemore, S.-J. & Mills, K. L. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65 , 187–207 (2014). This review presents adolescence as an important stage of development characterized by changes to social cognition.

Campbell, R. et al. Multiple risk behaviour in adolescence is associated with substantial adverse health and social outcomes in early adulthood: findings from a prospective birth cohort study. Prev. Med. 138 , 106157 (2020).

Kurten, S. et al. Like to drink: dynamics of liking alcohol posts and effects on alcohol use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 129 , 107145 (2022).

Vannucci, A., Simpson, E. G., Gagnon, S. & Ohannessian, C. M. Social media use and risky behaviors in adolescents: a meta‐analysis. J. Adolesc. 79 , 258–274 (2020).

Eichhorn, K. The End of Forgetting: Growing up with Social Media (Harvard Univ. Press, 2019).

Litt, E. & Hargittai, E. The imagined audience on social network sites. Soc. Media + Society 2 , 205630511663348 (2016).

Vitak, J. The impact of context collapse and privacy on social network site disclosures. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 56 , 451–470 (2012).

Livingstone, S. Taking risky opportunities in youthful content creation: teenagers’ use of social networking sites for intimacy, privacy and self-expression. N. Media Soc. 10 , 393–411 (2008).

Marciano, L., Schulz, P. J. & Camerini, A.-L. Cyberbullying perpetration and victimization in youth: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 25 , 163–181 (2020).

Mori, C., Temple, J. R., Browne, D. & Madigan, S. Association of sexting with sexual behaviors and mental health among adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 173 , 770 (2019).

Suler, J. The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7 , 321–326 (2004).

Wright, M. F., Harper, B. D. & Wachs, S. The associations between cyberbullying and callous-unemotional traits among adolescents: the moderating effect of online disinhibition. Pers. Individ. Differ. 140 , 41–45 (2019).

Nitschinsk, L., Tobin, S. J. & Vanman, E. J. The disinhibiting effects of anonymity increase online trolling. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 25 , 377–383 (2022).

Nadkarni, A. & Hofmann, S. G. Why do people use Facebook? Pers. Individ. Differ. 52 , 243–249 (2012).

Leary, M. R. & Kowalski, R. M. Impression management: a literature review and two-component model. Psychol. Bull. 107 , 34–47 (1990).

Zhao, S., Grasmuck, S. & Martin, J. Identity construction on Facebook: digital empowerment in anchored relationships. Comput. Hum. Behav. 24 , 1816–1836 (2008).

Bij de Vaate, N. A. J. D., Veldhuis, J. & Konijn, E. A. How online self-presentation affects well-being and body image: a systematic review. Telemat. Inform. 47 , 101316 (2020).

Reinecke, L. & Trepte, S. Authenticity and well-being on social network sites: a two-wave longitudinal study on the effects of online authenticity and the positivity bias in SNS communication. Comput. Hum. Behav. 30 , 95–102 (2014).

Twomey, C. & O’Reilly, G. Associations of self-presentation on Facebook with mental health and personality variables: a systematic review. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 20 , 587–595 (2017).

Vanden Abeele, M., Schouten, A. P. & Antheunis, M. L. Personal, editable, and always accessible: an affordance approach to the relationship between adolescents’ mobile messaging behavior and their friendship quality. J. Soc. Personal. Relatsh. 34 , 875–893 (2017).

Krause, H.-V., Baum, K., Baumann, A. & Krasnova, H. Unifying the detrimental and beneficial effects of social network site use on self-esteem: a systematic literature review. Media Psychol. 24 , 10–47 (2021).

Carr, C. T. & Foreman, A. C. Identity shift III: effects of publicness of feedback and relational closeness in computer-mediated communication. Media Psychol. 19 , 334–358 (2016).

Walther, J. B. et al. The effect of feedback on identity shift in computer-mediated communication. Media Psychol. 14 , 1–26 (2011).

Gonzales, A. L. & Hancock, J. T. Identity shift in computer-mediated environments. Media Psychol. 11 , 167–185 (2008).

Kelly, A. E. & Rodriguez, R. R. Publicly committing oneself to an identity. Basic. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28 , 185–191 (2006).

Petre, C. E. The relationship between Internet use and self-concept clarity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cyberpsychology 15 , https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2021-2-4 (2021).

Appel, M., Schreiner, C., Weber, S., Mara, M. & Gnambs, T. Intensity of Facebook use is associated with lower self-concept clarity: cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence. J. Media Psychol. 30 , 160–172 (2018).

Talaifar, S. & Lowery, B. S. Freedom and constraint in digital environments: implications for the self. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 18 , 544–575 (2022).

West, M., Rice, S. & Vella-Brodrick, D. Mid-adolescents’ social media use: supporting and suppressing autonomy. J. Adolesc. Res . https://doi.org/10.1177/07435584231168402 (2023).

Grasmuck, S., Martin, J. & Zhao, S. Ethno-racial identity displays on Facebook. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 15 , 158–188 (2009).

DeVito, M. A., Walker, A. M. & Birnholtz, J. ‘Too Gay for Facebook’: presenting LGBTQ+ identity throughout the personal social media ecosystem. Proc. ACM Hum.–Comput. Interact. 2 , 1–23 (2018).

Ellison, N., Heino, R. & Gibbs, E. Managing impressions online: self-presentation processes in the online dating environment. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun . 11 , https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00020.x (2006).

Hancock, J. T. in Oxford Handbook of Internet Psychology (eds Joinson, A. et al.) 287–301 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

Davidson, B. I. & Joinson, A. N. Shape shifting across social media. Soc. Media + Society 7 , 205630512199063 (2021).

Davis, J. L. Triangulating the self: identity processes in a connected era: triangulating the self. Symbolic Interaction 37 , 500–523 (2014).

Allen, B. J., Stratman, Z. E., Kerr, B. R., Zhao, Q. & Moreno, M. A. Associations between psychosocial measures and digital media use among transgender youth: cross-sectional study. JMIR Pediatr. Parent. 4 , e25801 (2021).

Haimson, O. L. Mapping gender transition sentiment patterns via social media data: toward decreasing transgender mental health disparities. J. Am. Med. Inform. Assoc. 26 , 749–758 (2019).

Harter, S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations (Guilford Press, 2012).

Crone, E. A., Green, K. H., van de Groep, I. H. & van der Cruijsen, R. A neurocognitive model of self-concept development in adolescence. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 4 , 273–295 (2022). This extensive review discusses how adolescence is an important time for self-concept development.

Pfeifer, J. H. & Peake, S. J. Self-development: integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Deve. Cognit. Neurosci. 2 , 55–69 (2012).

Sebastian, C., Burnett, S. & Blakemore, S.-J. Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends Cognit. Sci. 12 , 441–446 (2008).

Crocetti, E., Rubini, M., Luyckx, K. & Meeus, W. Identity formation in early and middle adolescents from various ethnic groups: from three dimensions to five statuses. J. Youth Adolesc. 37 , 983–996 (2008).

Morita, H., Griffioen, N. & Granic, I. in Handbook of Adolescent Digital Media Use and Mental Health (eds Nesi, J., Telzer, E. H. & Prinstein, M. J.) 63–84 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2022).

Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A. & Blakemore, S.-J. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Dev. Sci. 13 , 331–338 (2010).

Weil, L. G. et al. The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Conscious. Cogn. 22 , 264–271 (2013).

Moses-Payne, M. E., Chierchia, G. & Blakemore, S.-J. Age-related changes in the impact of valence on self-referential processing in female adolescents and young adults. Cognit. Dev. 61 , 101128 (2022).

Scheuplein, M. et al. Perspective taking and memory for self- and town-related information in male adolescents and young adults. Cognit. Dev. 67 , 101356 (2023).

Rodman, A. M., Powers, K. E. & Somerville, L. H. Development of self-protective biases in response to social evaluative feedback. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114 , 13158–13163 (2017).

Lee, A. Y., Mieczkowski, H., Ellison, N. B. & Hancock, J. T. The algorithmic crystal: conceptualizing the self through algorithmic personalization on TikTok. Proc. ACM Hum.–Comput. Interact. 6 , 1–22 (2022).

Thomaes, S. et al. I like me if you like me: on the interpersonal modulation and regulation of preadolescents’ state self-esteem. Child. Dev. 81 , 811–825 (2010).

Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J. & Schouten, A. P. Friend networking sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-esteem. CyberPsychol. Behav. 9 , 584–590 (2006).

Kwan, I. et al. Cyberbullying and children and young people’s mental health: a systematic map of systematic reviews. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 23 , 72–82 (2020).

Przybylski, A. K. & Bowes, L. Cyberbullying and adolescent well-being in England: a population-based cross-sectional study. Lancet Child. Adolesc. Health 1 , 19–26 (2017).

Peters, S. et al. Social media use and the not-so-imaginary audience: behavioral and neural mechanisms underlying the influence on self-concept. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 48 , 100921 (2021).

Wood, J. V. What is social comparison and how should we study it? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22 , 520–537 (1996).

Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L. & Suleiman, A. B. Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature 554 , 441–450 (2018).

Ferguson, A. M., Turner, G. & Orben, A. Social uncertainty in the digital world. Trends Cognit. Sci. 28 , 286–289 (2024).

Blease, C. R. Too many ‘friends,’ too few ‘likes’? Evolutionary psychology and ‘Facebook depression’. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 19 , 1–13 (2015).

Lee, H. Y. et al. Getting fewer “likes” than others on social media elicits emotional distress among victimized adolescents. Child. Dev. 91 , 2141–2159 (2020).

Nesi, J. & Prinstein, M. J. In search of likes: longitudinal associations between adolescents’ digital status seeking and health-risk behaviors. J. Clin. Child. Adolesc. Psychol. 48 , 740–748 (2019).

Carr, C. T., Hayes, R. A. & Sumner, E. M. Predicting a threshold of perceived Facebook post success via likes and reactions: a test of explanatory mechanisms. Commun. Res. Rep. 35 , 141–151 (2018).

Noon, E. J. & Meier, A. Inspired by friends: adolescents’ network homophily moderates the relationship between social comparison, envy, and inspiration on instagram. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 22 , 787–793 (2019).

Schreurs, L., Meier, A. & Vandenbosch, L. Exposure to the positivity bias and adolescents’ differential longitudinal links with social comparison, inspiration and envy depending on social media literacy. Curr. Psychol . https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03893-3 (2022).

Meier, A. & Krause, H.-V. Does passive social media use harm well-being? An adversarial review. J. Media Psychol. 35 , 169–180 (2023).

Nesi, J. & Prinstein, M. J. Using social media for social comparison and feedback-seeking: gender and popularity moderate associations with depressive symptoms. J. Abnorm. Child. Psychol. 43 , 1427–1438 (2015).

Lindström, B. et al. A computational reward learning account of social media engagement. Nat. Commun. 12 , 1311 (2021).

Fardouly, J., Diedrichs, P. C., Vartanian, L. R. & Halliwell, E. Social comparisons on social media: the impact of Facebook on young women’s body image concerns and mood. Body Image 13 , 38–45 (2015).

Scully, M., Swords, L. & Nixon, E. Social comparisons on social media: online appearance-related activity and body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls. Ir. J. Psychol. Med. 40 , 31–42 (2023).

Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L. & Crusius, J. The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 9 , 44–49 (2016).

Meier, A. & Johnson, B. K. Social comparison and envy on social media: a critical review. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 45 , 101302 (2022).

Verduyn, P., Gugushvili, N., Massar, K., Täht, K. & Kross, E. Social comparison on social networking sites. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 36 , 32–37 (2020).

Meier, A., Gilbert, A., Börner, S. & Possler, D. Instagram inspiration: how upward comparison on social network sites can contribute to well-being. J. Commun. 70 , 721–743 (2020).

Vaterlaus, J. M., Patten, E. V., Roche, C. & Young, J. A. #Gettinghealthy: the perceived influence of social media on young adult health behaviors. Comput. Hum. Behav. 45 , 151–157 (2015).

Valkenburg, P. M., Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., Van Driel, I. I. & Keijsers, L. Social media browsing and adolescent well-being: challenging the “passive social media use hypothesis”. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015 (2022).

Larson, R. W., Richards, M. H., Moneta, G., Holmbeck, G. & Duckett, E. Changes in adolescents’ daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18: disengagement and transformation. Dev. Psychol. 32 , 744–754 (1996).

Sebastian, C., Viding, E., Williams, K. D. & Blakemore, S.-J. Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cogn. 72 , 134–145 (2010).

Sebastian, C. et al. Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: implications of social neuroscience for education. NeuroImage 57 , 686–694 (2011).

Somerville, L. H. The teenage brain: sensitivity to social evaluation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22 , 121–127 (2013).

Larson, R. W. & How, U. S. Children and adolescents spend time: what it does (and doesn’t) tell us about their development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10 , 160–164 (2001).

Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R. & LaBar, K. S. Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Dev. Sci. 10 , 547–558 (2007).

Gunther Moor, B., van Leijenhorst, L., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Crone, E. A. & Van der Molen, M. W. Do you like me? Neural correlates of social evaluation and developmental trajectories. Soc. Neurosci. 5 , 461–482 (2010).

Silk, J. S. et al. Peer acceptance and rejection through the eyes of youth: pupillary, eyetracking and ecological data from the Chatroom Interact task. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 7 , 93–105 (2012).

Gao, S., Assink, M., Cipriani, A. & Lin, K. Associations between rejection sensitivity and mental health outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 57 , 59–74 (2017).

Prinstein, M. J., Nesi, J. & Telzer, E. H. Commentary: an updated agenda for the study of digital media use and adolescent development—future directions following Odgers & Jensen (2020). J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr. 61 , 349–352 (2020).

Meshi, D., Morawetz, C. & Heekeren, H. R. Nucleus accumbens response to gains in reputation for the self relative to gains for others predicts social media use. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7 , 1–11 (2013).

Crone, E. A. & Dahl, R. E. Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 , 636–650 (2012).

Platt, B., Kadosh, K. C. & Lau, J. Y. F. The role of peer rejection in adolescent depression. Depress. Anxiety 30 , 809–821 (2013).

Will, G.-J., Rutledge, R. B., Moutoussis, M. & Dolan, R. J. Neural and computational processes underlying dynamic changes in self-esteem. eLife 6 , e28098 (2017).

Macrynikola, N. & Miranda, R. Active Facebook use and mood: when digital interaction turns maladaptive. Comput. Hum. Behav. 97 , 271–279 (2019).

Grunewald, K., Deng, J., Wertz, J. & Schweizer, S. The effect of online social evaluation on mood and cognition in young people. Sci. Rep. 12 , 20999 (2022).

Andrews, J. L., Khin, A. C., Crayn, T., Humphreys, K. & Schweizer, S. Measuring online and offline social rejection sensitivity in the digital age. Psychol. Assess. 34 , 742–751 (2022).

Forest, A. L. & Wood, J. V. When social networking is not working: individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do not reap the benefits of self-disclosure on Facebook. Psychol. Sci. 23 , 295–302 (2012).

Valkenburg, P. M., Koutamanis, M. & Vossen, H. G. M. The concurrent and longitudinal relationships between adolescents’ use of social network sites and their social self-esteem. Comput. Hum. Behav. 76 , 35–41 (2017).

Burrow, A. L. & Rainone, N. How many likes did I get? purpose moderates links between positive social media feedback and self-esteem. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 69 , 232–236 (2017).

Seo, M., Kim, J. & Yang, H. Frequent interaction and fast feedback predict perceived social support: using crawled and self-reported data of Facebook users. J. Comput.-Mediat. Comm. 21 , 282–297 (2016).

Fuhrmann, D., Casey, C. S., Speekenbrink, M. & Blakemore, S.-J. Social exclusion affects working memory performance in young adolescent girls. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 40 , 100718 (2019).

Blakemore, S.-J. & Choudhury, S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiat 47 , 296–312 (2006).

Dreyfuss, M. et al. Teens impulsively react rather than retreat from threat. Dev. Neurosci. 36 , 220–227 (2014).

Guyer, A. E., Choate, V. R., Pine, D. S. & Nelson, E. E. Neural circuitry underlying affective response to peer feedback in adolescence. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 7 , 81–92 (2012).

Sherman, L. E., Payton, A. A., Hernandez, L. M., Greenfield, P. M. & Dapretto, M. The power of the like in adolescence: effects of peer influence on neural and behavioral responses to social media. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 1027–1035 (2016).

van Harmelen, A.-L. et al. Adolescent friendships predict later resilient functioning across psychosocial domains in a healthy community cohort. Psychol. Med. 47 , 2312–2322 (2017).

Chu, P. S., Saucier, D. A. & Hafner, E. Meta-analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children and adolescents. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 29 , 624–645 (2010).

Schneider, F. M. et al. Social media ostracism: the effects of being excluded online. Comput. Hum. Behav. 73 , 385–393 (2017).

Reich, S., Schneider, F. M. & Heling, L. Zero likes—symbolic interactions and need satisfaction online. Comput. Hum. Behav. 80 , 97–102 (2018).

Lutz, S. & Schneider, F. M. Is receiving dislikes in social media still better than being ignored? The effects of ostracism and rejection on need threat and coping responses online. Media Psychol. 24 , 741–765 (2021).

Lutz, S. Why don’t you answer me? Exploring the effects of (repeated exposure to) ostracism via messengers on users’ fundamental needs, well-being, and coping motivation. Media Psychol. 26 , 113–140 (2023).

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, C. T., Tan, E. S. H., Verlegh, P. W. J., Beyens, I. & Kühne, R. Don’t stress me now: assessing the regulatory impact of face-to-face and online feedback prosociality on stress during an important life event. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 25 , 307–327 (2020).

Trepte, S., Dienlin, T. & Reinecke, L. Influence of social support received in online and offline contexts on satisfaction with social support and satisfaction with life: a longitudinal study. Media Psychol. 18 , 74–105 (2015).

Dredge, R. & Schreurs, L. Social media use and offline interpersonal outcomes during youth: a systematic literature review. Mass. Commun. Soc. 23 , 885–911 (2020).

Colasante, T., Lin, L., De France, K. & Hollenstein, T. Any time and place? Digital emotional support for digital natives. Am. Psychol. 77 , 186–195 (2022).

Pouwels, J. L., Valkenburg, P. M., Beyens, I., Van Driel, I. I. & Keijsers, L. Social media use and friendship closeness in adolescents’ daily lives: an experience sampling study. Dev. Psychol. 57 , 309–323 (2021).

Mills, K. L. et al. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. NeuroImage 141 , 273–281 (2016).

Tamnes, C. K. et al. Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: a multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. J. Neurosci. 37 , 3402–3412 (2017).

Larsen, B. & Luna, B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 94 , 179–195 (2018).

Petanjek, Z. et al. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108 , 13281–13286 (2011).

Cohen, J. R. et al. A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors. Nat. Neurosci. 13 , 669–671 (2010).

Ernst, M. et al. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage 25 , 1279–1291 (2005).

Galván, A. & McGlennen, K. M. Enhanced striatal sensitivity to aversive reinforcement in adolescents versus adults. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 25 , 284–296 (2013).

Braams, B. R., Van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peper, J. S. & Crone, E. A. Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J. Neurosci. 35 , 7226–7238 (2015).

Schreuders, E. et al. Contributions of reward sensitivity to ventral striatum activity across adolescence and early adulthood. Child. Dev. 89 , 797–810 (2018).

Maza, M. T. et al. Association of habitual checking behaviors on social media with longitudinal functional brain development. JAMA Pediatr. 177 , 160–167 (2023).

Miller, J., Mills, K. L., Vuorre, M., Orben, A. & Przybylski, A. K. Impact of digital screen media activity on functional brain organization in late childhood: evidence from the ABCD study. Cortex 169 , 290–308 (2023).

Flayelle, M. et al. A taxonomy of technology design features that promote potentially addictive online behaviours. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2 , 136–150 (2023).

Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R. & Heim, C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 , 434–445 (2009).

Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D. & Griggs, C. Developmental changes in hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: normative changes and associations with puberty. Dev. Psychopathol. 21 , 69–85 (2009).

Somerville, L. H. et al. The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion in adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 24 , 1554–1562 (2013).

Stroud, L. R. et al. Stress response and the adolescent transition: performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev. Psychopathol. 21 , 47–68 (2009).

Avital, A. & Richter-Levin, G. Exposure to juvenile stress exacerbates the behavioural consequences of exposure to stress in the adult rat. Int. J. Neuropsychopharm. 8 , 163–173 (2005).

McCormick, C. M., Mathews, I. Z., Thomas, C. & Waters, P. Investigations of HPA function and the enduring consequences of stressors in adolescence in animal models. Brain Cogn. 72 , 73–85 (2010).

Eiland, L. & Romeo, R. D. Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience 249 , 162–171 (2013).

Romeo, R. D. The teenage brain. Curr. Direc. Psychol. Sci. 22 , 140–145 (2013).

Afifi, T. D., Zamanzadeh, N., Harrison, K. & Acevedo Callejas, M. WIRED: the impact of media and technology use on stress (cortisol) and inflammation (interleukin IL-6) in fast paced families. Comput. Hum. Behav. 81 , 265–273 (2018).

Morin-Major, J. K. et al. Facebook behaviors associated with diurnal cortisol in adolescents: is befriending stressful? Psychoneuroendocrinology 63 , 238–46 (2016).

Ghai, S. It’s time to reimagine sample diversity and retire the WEIRD dichotomy. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5 , 971–972 (2021).

Munafò, M. R. & Davey Smith, G. Robust research needs many lines of evidence. Nature 553 , 399–401 (2018).

Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S. & Johnson, K. L. The fundamental importance of method to theory. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2 , 55–66 (2022).

Parry, D. A., Fisher, J. T., Mieczkowski, H., Sewall, C. J. R. & Davidson, B. I. Social media and well-being: a methodological perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 45 , 101285 (2022).

Will, G.-J. et al. Neurocomputational mechanisms underpinning aberrant social learning in young adults with low self-esteem. Transl. Psychiatry 10 , 96 (2020).

Walther, J. B. Affordances, effects, and technology errors. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 36 , 190–193 (2013).

Piray, P. & Daw, N. D. A model for learning based on the joint estimation of stochasticity and volatility. Nat. Commun. 12 , 6587 (2021).

Bronfenbrenner, U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design (Harvard Univ. Press, 1979).

Slater, M. D. Reinforcing spirals: the mutual influence of media selectivity and media effects and their impact on individual behavior and social identity. Commun. Theory 17 , 281–303 (2007).

Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J. & Walther, J. B. Media effects: theory and research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67 , 315–338 (2016).

Aalbers, G., McNally, R. J., Heeren, A., De Wit, S. & Fried, E. I. Social media and depression symptoms: a network perspective. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148 , 1454–1462 (2019).

Ghai, S., Fassi, L., Awadh, F. & Orben, A. Lack of sample diversity in research on adolescent depression and social media use: a scoping review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 11 , 759–772 (2023).

Cramer, A. O. J. et al. Major depression as a complex dynamic system. PLoS ONE 11 , e0167490 (2016).

Kendler, K. S., Zachar, P. & Craver, C. What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders? Psychol. Med. 41 , 1143–1150 (2011).

van de Leemput, I. A. et al. Critical slowing down as early warning for the onset and termination of depression. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 111 , 87–92 (2014).

Trepte, S. The social media privacy model: privacy and communication in the light of social media affordances. Commun. Theory 31 , 549–570 (2021).

Reinecke, L. et al. Permanently online and permanently connected: development and validation of the Online Vigilance Scale. PLoS ONE 13 , e0205384 (2018).

Trieu, P., Bayer, J. B., Ellison, N. B., Schoenebeck, S. & Falk, E. Who likes to be reachable? Availability preferences, weak ties, and bridging social capital. Inform. Commun. Soc. 22 , 1096–1111 (2019).

Daft, R. L. & Lengel, R. H. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Manag. Sci. 32 , 554–571 (1986).

Rhee, L., Bayer, J. B., Lee, D. S. & Kuru, O. Social by definition: how users define social platforms and why it matters. Telemat. Inform. 59 , 101538 (2021).

Valkenburg, P. M. Understanding self-effects in social media: self-effects in social media. Hum. Commun. Res. 43 , 477–490 (2017).

Thorson, K. & Wells, C. Curated flows: a framework for mapping media exposure in the digital age: curated flows. Commun. Theor. 26 , 309–328 (2016).

Zhao, H. & Wagner, C. How TikTok leads users to flow experience: investigating the effects of technology affordances with user experience level and video length as moderators. INTR 33 , 820–849 (2023).

Carr, C. T., Wohn, D. Y. & Hayes, R. A. As social support: relational closeness, automaticity, and interpreting social support from paralinguistic digital affordances in social media. Comput. Hum. Behav. 62 , 385–393 (2016).

Rice, R. E. et al. Organizational media affordances: operationalization and associations with media use: organizational media affordances. J. Commun. 67 , 106–130 (2017).

Scissors, L., Burke, M. & Wengrovitz, S. in Proc. 19th ACM Conf. Computer-Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing—CSCW ’16 1499–1508 (ACM Press, 2016).

Boyd, D. M. in A Networked Self: Identity, Community and Culture in Social Networking Sites (ed. Papacharissi, Z.) 35–58 (Routledge, 2011).

Valkenburg, P. M. in Handbook of Adolescent Digital Media Use and Mental Health (eds Nesi, J., Telzer, E. H. & Prinstein, M. J.) 39–60 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2022).

Dennis, Fuller & Valacich, Media Tasks, and communication processes: a theory of media synchronicity. MIS Q. 32 , 575 (2008).

DeAndrea, D. C. Advancing warranting theory: advancing warranting theory. Commun. Theor. 24 , 186–204 (2014).

Uhlhaas, P. J. et al. Towards a youth mental health paradigm: a perspective and roadmap. Mol. Psychiatry 28 , 3171–3181 (2023).

Kachuri, L. et al. Principles and methods for transferring polygenic risk scores across global populations. Nat. Rev. Genet. 25 , 8–25 (2024).

Weinstein, E. C. & Selman, R. L. Digital stress: adolescents’ personal accounts. N. Media Soc. 18 , 391–409 (2016).

Steele, R. G., Hall, J. A. & Christofferson, J. L. Conceptualizing digital stress in adolescents and young adults: toward the development of an empirically based model. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 23 , 15–26 (2020).

Nick, E. A. et al. Adolescent digital stress: frequencies, correlates, and longitudinal association with depressive symptoms. J. Adolesc. Health 70 , 336–339 (2022).

Van Der Schuur, W. A., Baumgartner, S. E. & Sumter, S. R. Social media use, social media stress, and sleep: examining cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships in adolescents. Health Commun. 34 , 552–559 (2019).

Fabio, S. & Sonja, P. Is cyberbullying worse than traditional bullying? Examining the differential roles of medium, publicity, and anonymity for the perceived severity of bullying. J. Youth Adolesc. 42 , 739–750 (2013).

Tokunaga, R. S. Following you home from school: a critical review and synthesis of research on cyberbullying victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26 , 277–287 (2010).

Khetawat, D. & Steele, R. G. Examining the association between digital stress components and psychological wellbeing: a meta-analysis. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 26 , 957–974 (2023).

Beyens, I., Frison, E. & Eggermont, S. “I don’t want to miss a thing”: adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Comput. Hum. Behav. 64 , 1–8 (2016).

Wartberg, L., Thomasius, R. & Paschke, K. The relevance of emotion regulation, procrastination, and perceived stress for problematic social media use in a representative sample of children and adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 121 , 106788 (2021).

Winstone, L., Mars, B., Haworth, C. M. A. & Kidger, J. Types of social media use and digital stress in early adolescence. J. Early Adolescence 43 , 294–319 (2023).

West, M., Rice, S. & Vella-Brodrick, D. Exploring the “social” in social media: adolescent relatedness—thwarted and supported. J. Adolesc. Res . https://doi.org/10.1177/07435584211062158 (2021).

Gilbert, A., Baumgartner, S. E. & Reinecke, L. Situational boundary conditions of digital stress: goal conflict and autonomy frustration make smartphone use more stressful. Mob. Media Commun . https://doi.org/10.1177/20501579221138017 (2022).

Freytag, A. et al. Permanently online—always stressed out? The effects of permanent connectedness on stress experiences. Hum. Commun. Res. 47 , 132–165 (2021).

Johannes, N. et al. The relationship between online vigilance and affective well-being in everyday life: combining smartphone logging with experience sampling. Media Psychol. 24 , 581–605 (2021).

Reinecke, L. et al. Digital stress over the life span: the effects of communication load and internet multitasking on perceived stress and psychological health impairments in a german probability sample. Media Psychol. 20 , 90–115 (2017).

Schönbach, K. in The International Encyclopedia of Media Effects (eds Rössler, P., Hoffner, C. A. & Zoonen, L.) 1–11 (Wiley, 2017).

Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L. & Evans, T. W. Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63 , 119–132 (1992).

Ferster, C. B. A functional analysis of depression. Am. Psychol. 28 , 857–870 (1973).

Carvalho, J. P. & Hopko, D. R. Behavioral theory of depression: reinforcement as a mediating variable between avoidance and depression. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 42 , 154–162 (2011).

Helbig-Lang, S. & Petermann, F. Tolerate or eliminate? A systematic review on the effects of safety behavior across anxiety disorders. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 17 , 218–233 (2010).

Marciano, L., Driver, C. C., Schulz, P. J. & Camerini, A.-L. Dynamics of adolescents’ smartphone use and well-being are positive but ephemeral. Sci. Rep. 12 , 1316 (2022).

Rao, P. A. et al. Social anxiety disorder in childhood and adolescence: descriptive psychopathology. Behav. Res. Ther. 45 , 1181–1191 (2007).

Corning, A. F., Krumm, A. J. & Smitham, L. A. Differential social comparison processes in women with and without eating disorder symptoms. J. Couns. Psychol. 53 , 338–349 (2006).

Radovic, A., Gmelin, T., Stein, B. D. & Miller, E. Depressed adolescents’ positive and negative use of social media. J. Adolesc. 55 , 5–15 (2017).

Download references

Acknowledgements

A.O. and T.D. were funded by the Medical Research Council (MC_UU_00030/13). A.O. was funded by the Jacobs Foundation and a UKRI Future Leaders Fellowship (MR/X034925/1). S.-J.B. is funded by Wellcome (grant numbers WT107496/Z/15/Z and WT227882/Z/23/Z), the MRC, the Jacobs Foundation, the Wellspring Foundation and the University of Cambridge.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Amy Orben & Tim Dalgleish

School of Business, Economics and Society, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen–Nürnberg, Nürnberg, Germany

Adrian Meier

Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

A.O. conceptualized the manuscript; A.O and A.M wrote the original draft; A.O., A.M., T.D. and S.-J.B. reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed substantially to discussion of the content, and reviewed and/or edited the manuscript before submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Orben .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information.

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Emily Weinstein, who co-reviewed with Beck Tench; Nastasia Griffioen; and Margarita Panayiotou for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Orben, A., Meier, A., Dalgleish, T. et al. Mechanisms linking social media use to adolescent mental health vulnerability. Nat Rev Psychol (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00307-y

Download citation

Accepted : 02 April 2024

Published : 07 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00307-y

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

research article social psychology

Psychology Headlines

From around the world.

  • OpenAI Launches GPT-o, With New Humanlike Capabilities
  • Illness Took Away Her Voice. AI Cloned a Replica She Carries in Her Phone
  • Scientists Unveil Most Detailed Map of the Human Brain Ever Created
  • AI Deadbots Spur Call for Safeguards to Prevent Unwanted "Hauntings"
  • AI Knowledge Gets Your Foot in the Door, Study Finds
  • How a 1973 Missouri Law Makes It Hard for Pregnant Women to Divorce
  • AI Systems Are Already Skilled at Deceiving and Manipulating Humans
  • Eating Disorders Common in People with Type 1 Diabetes

Source: Psychology News Center

research article social psychology

The moderating role of psychological power distance on the relationship between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion

  • Open access
  • Published: 14 May 2024

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

research article social psychology

  • Yavuz Korkmazyurek   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-8329-4080 1 &
  • Metin Ocak   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1142-3661 1  

Destructive leadership, a prevalent negative behavior in modern organizations, continues to captivate the interest of scholars and professionals due to its detrimental aftermath. Drawing from social psychological (culture) and conservation of resources theory, we explore the moderating impact of psychological power distance on the link between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion. The main contribution of this study is that it has created new information about the moderating role of some specific sub-dimensions of psychological power distance (e.g., hierarchy, prestige) in the relationship between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion. Our findings also reveal a positive correlation between a destructive leadership style and emotional exhaustion. Furthermore, the prestige aspect of psychological power distance amplifies the influence of deficient leadership abilities and unethical conduct on emotional exhaustion. Notably, our study highlights that in the Turkish context, characterized by high power distance, and escalating hierarchies the impact of nepotism disparities on emotional exhaustion. In conclusion, these novel insights underscore a significant research avenue regarding cultural facets.

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Introduction

The actions of leaders in charge of societies and organizations have far-reaching effects, influencing organizational culture and the mental well-being of employees. On the other hand, culture is also recognized to influence various organizational relationships. Concordantly, one cultural element that can explain variations in leadership effectiveness, work attitudes, or job performance is an employee’s power distance orientation (Leonidou et al., 2021 ; Matta et al., 2022 ). Power distance at an individual level also serves as a moderating factor on various aspects, such as the effectiveness of leadership, employees’ perceptions and opinions of their organizations, and the core impacts of HR practices on employees (e.g., Adamovic, 2023 ; Li et al., 2017 ; Loi et al., 2012 ). In this context, despite some recent studies exploring the nature of power and its effects on leader behavior and employee responses from a psychological standpoint (e.g., Kelemen et al., 2020 ; Liao et al., 2021 ; Peng et al., 2021 ), given the extensive influence of psychological power, it is challenging to assert that the literature has fully developed.

According to the Conservation of Resources (COR) theory, resources are broadly defined as objects, personal characteristics, conditions, or energies that are valued because they help one to either directly obtain his or her goals or thwart his or her goal-relevant tendencies (Hobfoll, 2011 ). However, some non-constructive leadership behaviors such as DL continue to threaten the motivation level of employees (Rasid et al., 2013 ), individual resources (Hobfoll, 2011 ), and the welfare of organizations (Brouwers & Paltu, 2020 ; Veldsman, 2012 ). This fact creates a need to understand the contextual development of DL in organizations. DL can be seen as a new type of leadership where leaders engage in systematic and prolonged psychological abuse of subordinates (Ryan et al., 2021 ). Previous research has indicated that the impact of DL may depend on the context, as relationships can vary based on cultural and situational factors (e.g., Burns, 2021 ; Fors Brandebo, 2020 ). In parallel, numerous studies reveal the consequences of such leadership styles (Tepper, 2000 ) and propose theoretical models explaining the mechanisms of these styles (Einarsen et al., 2007 ; Wang et al., 2010 ). More specifically, negative leadership styles have been associated in the current literature with Emotional Exhaustion (EE) (Gkorezis et al., 2015 ; Koç et al., 2022 ), turnover intentions (Badar et al., 2023 ), and counterproductive work behavior (Murad et al., 2021 ). Besides, there is also an increasing research trend regarding the roles played by leadership in the moods and emotions of subordinates (Bono et al., 2007 ; Gooty et al., 2010 ).

“National culture has a crucial role in influencing the occurrence of leadership style” (Zhang & Liao, 2015 , p. 960), and shapes subordinates’ reactions toward these leadership styles (Hofstede, 2001 ; Tepper et al., 2017 ). One of these leadership styles is Destructive Leadership (DL) which is increasing in today’s societal and business areas (Krasikova et al., 2013 ) and prevents proper organizational functioning (Cascio & Aguinis, 2008 ). Organizations invest substantial resources in safeguarding and enhancing employee well-being (Salas-Vallina et al., 2021 ). Within this framework, COR, rooted in the resource-based perspectives of organizations, underscores individuals’ endeavors to safeguard, uphold, and cultivate their resources, highlighting bad management and stress as perceived threats to these resources (Hobfoll, 2011 ). Therefore, based on the COR, enhancing our understanding of the phenomenon of DL by exploring the impact of Psychological Power Distance (PPD) experienced by subordinates on their perceptions of leaders’ abusive behaviors is also significant in terms of decreasing employees’ level of Emotional Exhaustion (EE). EE, is expressed as “feelings of being emotionally drained by one’s work” (Bakker & Costa, 2014 , p. 2), and one of the primary emotional states experienced by employees today. In conclusion, the COR theory as a factor in work-related stress and destructive leadership can be used as a basis to eradicate the harmful effects of destructive leadership for the betterment of professional environments.

As a cultural factor, “Power Distance” (PD) assesses the probability that individuals facing greater inequality within the same social framework will recognize and expect unequal power distribution (Gonzalez, 2021 ). Hence, PD is a distinguishing feature among societies (Meydan et al., 2014 ). In this line, the PD beliefs of subordinates also vary depending on different leadership styles (Yang, 2020 ). For instance, in some cultures, leaders garner respect for taking decisive action, while in others, collaborative and participative decision-making methods hold more significance (Ahmad et al., 2021 , p. 1112). However, the issue of low reliability persists in many power distance scales (Taras, 2014 ). In this context, Adamovic ( 2023 ) contends that the measurement components of the Psychological Power Distance (PPD) scale he created amalgamate a broad power distance aspect and encompass noteworthy, though distinct, facets of power distance. Besides, although the concepts of hierarchy and power are often used as substitute concepts, the distinction between these two concepts has been significantly neglected in previous research (Aïssaoui & Fabian, 2015 ). For example, “in France, employees often do not tolerate power differences, but they tend to value a strong hierarchy” (Adamovic, 2023 , p. 3; d’Iribarne, 1996 ).

However, empirical research on the effects of psychological power distance on DL and related outcomes such as employee EE is surprisingly scarce. This paper seeks to address this gap in the literature. Thus, we aim to explore the potential moderating impact of the newly defined PPD on the connection between DL and EE to achieve trustworthy empirical findings. On the other hand, the study specifically focuses on a sample of Turkish employees, given that Türkiye is the largest economy in the Middle East and Turkish cultural values have had a profound impact on the way organizations are managed in the Middle East region. Besides, most countries in the Middle East were founded with the dissolution of the Ottoman Empire and are societies that come from the same traditions and customs (Lindholm, 2008 ). Thus, as a society that traditionally values respect and compliance with authority, Türkiye represents an ideal context to study the effects of psychological power distance.

Theoretical background

Destructive leadership.

“Destructive leadership is conceptualized as a broad umbrella” (Mackey et al., 2021 , p. 707) that ranges from abusive supervision (Tepper, 2000 ; Tepper et al., 2017 ) to overburdening followers (Schmid et al., 2019 ). Therefore, a wide variety of theories and different approaches, such as the Psychodynamic approach (Pillay & April, 2022 ) or Strain theory (Chen & Cheung, 2020 ) which is a criminological theory, have been used to explain the behaviors and effects of Destructive Leadership (DL). On the other hand, researchers have discovered that a rise in disruptive behaviors, which can deplete individuals’ psychological resources, may stem from factors like heightened anxiety (Byrne et al., 2014 ), work-related stress (Rosenstein, 2017 ), or excessive job pressure (Lam et al., 2017 ). Concurrently, the field of DL is experiencing increased diversity. Within this realm, DL manifests in various structural forms. As per Einarsen et al. ( 2007 ) and Larsson et al. ( 2012 ), these forms can be categorized as active or passive. Active behaviors encompass traits such as arrogance, unfairness, and intimidating or disciplining subordinates. Passive behavioral patterns highlight leader qualities like disinterest, avoidance of conflict, or poor planning skills (Larsson et al., 2012 ). Active behaviors are systematic and deliberate, while passive forms indicate deficiencies in leaders’ work and responsibilities (Einarsen et al., 2007 ). In addition, DL is divided into two dimensions in the literature: task and relationship. Task-related behaviors represent perceptions of the leader’s competence, including:

Isolation from outside interference and excessive control.

Lack of determination and uncertainty.

Stress and loss of control.

The relationship-related behaviors dimension refers to the leader’s skills in human relations, such as:

Low ability to relate to colleagues and subordinates and lack of job satisfaction.

Lack of understanding and self-centered behavior (Fors et al., 2016 ).

On the other hand, “employees will attribute leadership behavior in the process of interaction with the leaders” (Jiao & Wang, 2023 , p. 2), and the psychological states of subordinates will also be affected depending on their attribution. As a result, based on attribution theory (Heider, 1958 ; Weiner, 1985 ), it should not be ignored that whether the above-mentioned behaviors will be perceived as destructive or non-destructive may differ depending on the psychological state and perceptions of the subordinates in addition to cultural impact (Kong & Jogaratnam, 2007 ; Ojo, 2012 ).

Emotional exhaustion

Burnout, a psychological syndrome brought on by a prolonged reaction to ongoing workplace stressors (Maslach et al., 2001 ), is a significant issue that is becoming worse as workers are subjected to increasing pressure and demands from their managers under different cultural contexts (Rattrie et al., 2020 ). Moreover, burnout has been associated with several negative organizational outcomes, including job performance, emotional labor, and reduced employee well-being (Moon & Hur, 2011 ; Qiu et al., 2023 ; Maslach et al., 2001 ). “It is generally accepted to encompass three dimensions that occur in a developmental sequence” (Strack et al., 2015 , p. 578): from emotional exhaustion (EE) to depersonalization and subsequent decline in achievement (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993 ; Maslach et al., 2001 ). Within this framework, EE refers to the extent to which an individual is depleted or lacking in physical and psychological resources to cope with an interpersonal stress situation (Maslach et al., 2001 ). Employees who experience EE at work feel extremely stressed because they lose their physical and mental endurance (Obi et al., 2020 ) which ultimately leads to unhealthy tendencies as well as anxiety, stress, and depression (Bianchi et al., 2015 ; Weigl et al., 2017 ). On the other hand, the main characteristics of EE at the organizational level are the desire to quit work, absenteeism, and low morale (Maslach, 1996 ). Ultimately, the chronic experience of negative emotions in both individual and organizational contexts and the difficulties employees experience in regulating them can deplete their cognitive and emotional resources, which emerges as an important risk factor for EE (Chang, 2009 ; Hsieh et al., 2011 ).

Destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion

Different leadership styles have a known impact on employees’ emotions (Baig et al., 2021 ), and “employees’ perceptions about the leader are likely to affect their attitudes” (Gkorezis et al., 2015 , p. 622). In this context, destructive leadership styles (e.g., abusive supervision, petty tyranny, negative leadership) may trigger negative emotional reactions from employees (Schilling & Schyns, 2015 ), and increase employees’ emotional exhaustion (Chi & Liang, 2013 ). According to the Emotional Dissonance theory, negative supervision may also lead employees to conceal their true emotions (Naseer & Raja, 2021 ). Thus, scholars are focusing on leaders’ negative behavioral impact on employees’ emotional exhaustion levels (Gkorezis et al., 2015 ) to increase employee well-being at work (Hetrick et al., 2022 ). In addition, interpersonal stressors that diminish the well-being of employees are frequently experienced within the organizational atmosphere dominated by DL due to the nature of this harmful style (Hetrick et al., 2022 ). According to the Emotional Dissonance theory, negative supervision may also lead employees to conceal their true emotions (Naseer & Raja, 2021 ). Within this framework, scholars are focusing on leaders’ negative behavioral impact on employees’ emotional exhaustion levels (Gkorezis et al., 2015 ) to increase employee well-being at work (Hetrick et al., 2022 ), interpersonal stressors that diminish the well-being of employees are frequently experienced within the organizational atmosphere dominated by DL due to the nature of this harmful style (Hetrick et al., 2022 ), this article bases the theoretical connection between Destructive Leadership (DL) and Emotional Exhaustion (EM) on the definition of Einarsen et al. ( 2007 ).

[..] is the systematic and repeated behavior of a leader or manager that harms the organization’s legitimate interests by undermining the organization’s resources, and effectiveness, motivation, and job satisfaction of subordinates (p. 208).

Current studies have explored the positive relationship between despotic, toxic, and destructive leadership with emotional exhaustion (e.g., Shahzad et al., 2023 ; Koç et al., 2022 ). In this context, “meta-analytic evidence demonstrates that DL has negative consequences for followers’ workplace behaviors (e.g., job performance, organizational citizenship behaviors [OCBs], workplace deviance)” (Mackey et al., 2019 , p. 3). These empirical findings may suggest that DL outputs could also result in EM among employees. In conclusion, destructive leaders can cause fundamental problems in business life, such as increasing the level of emotional exhaustion (Krumov et al., 2016 ), and subordinates who are constantly exposed to leaders’ destructive practices experience frustration and emotional exhaustion (Glasø & Vie, 2009 ). Given the theoretical discussions above, the research’s first hypothesis was formulated as follows.

H1: There is a positive relationship between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion.

Psychological power distance

The PPD concept originates from a multidisciplinary field of study called cross-cultural psychology, which seeks to understand how culture impacts the cognitive and behavioral outcomes of individuals and groups (Yang, 2020 ). According to Hofstede ( 1991 , p. 27), “power distance can be described as the degree to which individuals who are less powerful within a country’s institutions and organizations anticipate and acknowledge the unequal distribution of power”. In this context, “Shore and Cross ( 2005 , p. 57) underlined that power is distributed more equitably in low power distance cultures and unequally in high power distance cultures. For instance, the power distance index (Khakhar & Rammal, 2013 ) shows that the Arab world, which values traditional authority highly (Inglehart, 1997 ), scores highly in this index, and people working in these cultures strictly follow higher hierarchical orders (Chiaburu et al., 2015 ; Korkmazyurek & Korkmazyurek, 2023 ). In summary, “individuals who score highly on psychological power distance also tend 1) accept and tolerate power differences in the workplace, 2) avoid conflict with authority figures, 3) prefer a clear hierarchy at work, 4) strive for status and prestige, and 5) expect a social distance between managers and employees.” (Adamovic, 2023 , p. 2).

Psychological power distance as a moderator

It has been suggested that various cultures have their norms regarding what constitutes good or bad leadership, and these norms may be reflected in the perception of psychological power distance (Tang et al., 2020 ). In this context, numerous studies explore the connection between PD and leaders’ influence tactics as PD decides if subordinates in a culture would accept a leader’s influence and the specific situations in which a leader might face resistance from a group of subordinates. Thus, investigating the role of PD in employees’ perception of leaders and better understanding the impact of leaders on employee well-being, will not only inform practices for workplace health intervention but also enlighten leadership researchers in discussing the universal and contingency theory of leadership. On the other hand, several power distance measures, like the ones created by Cable & Edwards ( 2004 ), Dorfman & Howell ( 1988 ), and Maznevski and colleagues ( 2002 ), have produced interesting findings on the importance of power distance concerning employee results and leadership (Adamovic, 2023 , p. 2). As an example, Tepper ( 2007 ) claims that “countries with high power distance experience more abusive supervision”. Thus, individuals characterized by large power distance have a high tolerance for lack of autonomy and rely more on centralization and formalization of authority (Hofstede, 1980 ). In this context, PPD influences how people feel, think, and act about problems of status and power at work and is crucial in understanding how leaders and subordinates interact (Adamovic, 2023 , p. 1).

The moderating impact of PD on the link between workers’ job satisfaction, performance, and absenteeism was highlighted by Lam and Friends ( 2002 . p.14). On the other hand, Farh and Friends ( 2007 : 721) found in their study that PD had a negative moderating effect on the relationship between work outcomes such as organizational commitment, job performance, and conscientiousness. According to the findings above, we can argue that PPD has a deterministic effect on the functioning of the theoretical mechanisms between DL and EM. Besides, In countries with high power distance, abuse by superiors is quite normative and consistent for subordinates in superior-subordinate relationships (Tepper, 2007 ). In this regard, the need for power in the prestige dimension (Carl et al., 2004 ; Hofstede, 2001 ; Schwartz, 2014 ), which is an organic extension of previous power distance studies, is also associated with narcissistic and Machiavellian actions and attitudes (Jonason et al., 2022 ). “People with high power distance orientation in the workplace typically accept status disparities, whereas people with low power distance orientation frequently support treating everyone equally regardless of status symbols.” (Adamovic, 2023 , p. 3). Conversely, workers with a low power distance orientation favor participatory leadership and decision-making processes which is not as prevalent under abusive supervision (Rao & Pearce, 2016 ). As a result, in countries with low power distance, abusive supervision may affect the emotional state of subordinates (Meydan et al., 2014 ).

De Clercq and colleagues ( 2021 ) discovered that PPD is positively associated with subordinates’ perception of superiors’ destructive leadership. This indicates that when subordinates perceive high PPD, they are more likely to view their leaders as engaging in such destructive behaviors. This correlation can also be attributed to abusive supervision, a form of destructive leadership behavior. According to social learning theory (Rumjaun & Narod, 2020 ), when leaders’ power is internalized and reflected as subordinates’ psychological power distance, the aggressive behaviors displayed by leaders are likely to be observed and learned by subordinates. These aggressive behaviors could then lead to emotionally exhausting reactions in the subordinates. Correlationally, this study suggests that psychological power distance mediates the link between destructive leadership behaviors and subordinates’ emotional exhaustion levels.

H2: Psychological power distance has a moderating role in the relationship between perceived destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion.

The causal research method which is one of the quantitative research methods is used in the study. Cross-sectional data were collected using an electronic survey form through the convenience sampling method. Participants received the link to the electronic survey form via social media and email. The statistical analyses were carried out with AMOS 24 and SPSS 27.

Sample and procedure

The survey sample size was determined by the Non-random convenience sampling method and the process of its determination was as follows: The sample size that can numerically represent the universe of working people was calculated with the formula below (Ding et al., 2022 ).

In this formula, n represents the required sample size and Z represents the z-statistic at a 90% confidence level (Z = 1.64). σ represents the standard deviation of the overall population and takes the value of 0.5. d is the tolerance error or sampling error. It is the difference between the universe parameter and the statistical value obtained from the sample. Since such a research model has not been studied before in the Turkish culture, the tolerance error for the sample was accepted as 10%. The final required sample size was calculated as 67.

Along this line, in a homogenous group with a reliability of 0.90 and a sampling error of 0.10, a sample group of 61 people can represent a universe of 100 million people (Yazıcıoğlu & Erdoğan, 2004 ). Moreover, in a heterogeneous group, a sample size of 96 is sufficient. Data were collected from a total of 222 employees working in different jobs by using the convenience sampling method via an online survey. This sample strategy allows us to collect data that covers more industries in Türkiye. The participation of participants in the research was voluntary. Working in a workplace was the only criterion for participants. In this context, it was accepted that the sample size was large enough to represent the universe.

117 (53%) of the participants were female, and 105 (47%) were male. The participants have 14.48 (sd = 10.46) mean years of working experience, while their age was between 19 and 67 years, with a mean value of 40.87 (sd = 9.42). % 24.9 of the participants were between 19 and 34, % 24.9 between 35 and 40, % 25.8 between 41 and 47, and % 24,4 between 47 and 67 years old.

Measure of psychological power distance

The PPD perception was measured with the scale, developed by Adamovic ( 2023 ). This scale is a five-point Likert-type scale (1 strongly disagree to 5 strongly agree) comprising fifteen items under five factors (Power, Conflict with Authority Figure, Hierarchy, Prestige, and Social Distance). Ascending numbers indicate the extent to which power distance was perceived. The overall original psychological power distance scale demonstrated strong reliability (α = 0.82). The scale has not been used in Turkish before. For this reason, firstly, this scale was adapted to Turkish, and then the validity and reliability of the scale were tested. By adopting this scale, the method suggested by Brislin et al. ( 1973 ) was used. This method includes five basic steps: translation into the target language, evaluation of the translation into the target language, back-translation into the source language, evaluation of the back-translation into the source language, and final evaluation with experts. After the adaptation process, exploratory factor analysis was applied for the validity of the scale. In the exploratory factor analysis, the Principal Axis Analysis method and the Varimax Rotation Technique were applied to calculate factor loadings. Factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 were taken into consideration. As a result of the factor analysis, it was seen that all factor loadings were higher than 0.30 and there were no overlapping items. The lowest factor loading value recommended for a good factor analysis is 0.30 (Tavakol & Wetzel, 2020 ). As a result of the exploratory factor analysis, the KMO (Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin) value was found to be 0.76, and the result of Bartlett’s test was found to be p  < 0.001. After that, Confirmatory Factor Analysis was performed to examine the structural validity of the measurement tool. The single-factor, first-level related, unrelated, and second-level related models were tested and Psychological Power Distance Scale showed the highest goodness of fit in the first-level related model (Δχ2 = 145.18, p  < 0.001, SD = 79, Δχ2/SD = 1.84, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.92, IFI = 0.92, TLI = 0,90) which verified it’s original five-factor dimension. In our study, the Psychological Power Distance scale showed generally strong reliability (α = 0.78).

Measurement of destructive leadership

Destructive Leadership was measured with the scale which was developed by Aydinay ( 2022 ). Five Point Likert-type scale (1 strongly disagree to 5 strongly agree) comprises 26 items under five factors (Inadequate leadership skills and unethical behaviors, Authoritarian leadership, Inability to deal with new technology and other changes, Nepotism (favoritism), Callousness toward subordinates). Ascending numbers indicate the extent to which destructive leadership was perceived. The original scale’s reliability was reported using Cronbach’s coefficient alpha of α = 0.97, which showed that the scale was reliable. The validity of the scale was tested with confirmatory factor analysis, (Δχ2 = 590.23, p  < 0.01, SD = 280, Δχ2/SD = 2.11, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.94, IFI = 0.94, TLI = 0,93) which verified it’s original five-factor dimension. In our study, the Destructive Leadership scale showed generally strong reliability (α = 0.97).

Measurement of emotional exhaustion

In the study, to measure the emotional exhaustion levels the emotional exhaustion dimension scale in the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MTE) (Maslach et al. 1996 ), translated into Turkish by Ergin ( 1992 ), was used. Five-point Likert-type scale (1 strongly disagree to 5 strongly agree) comprises 9 items under one factor. The original scale’s reliability was reported using Cronbach’s coefficient alpha of α = 0.86, which showed that the scale was reliable. The validity of the scale was tested with confirmatory factor analysis, (Δχ2 = 40.51, p  < 0.006, SD = 21, Δχ2/SD = 1.93, RMSEA = 0.07, GFI = 0.96, CFI = 0.98, IFI = 0.98, TLI = 0,97) which verified it’s original one-factor dimension. In our study, the Emotional Exhaustion scale showed generally strong reliability (α = 0.91).

Table  1 displays the variables’ descriptive statistics as well as the Pearson correlation coefficients. Examining the correlations between the variables of the study, all sub-dimensions of Destructive Leadership [Inadequate leadership skills and unethical behaviors ( r  = 0.46, p  < 0.01), Authoritarian leadership ( r  = 0.49, p  < 0.01), Inability to deal with new technology and other changes ( r  = 0.42, p  < 0.01), Nepotism ( r  = 0.40, p  < 0.01), Callousness toward subordinates ( r  = 0.40, p  < 0.01)] were positively correlated with emotional exhaustion. There was no correlation between Psychological Power Distance sub-dimensions (Power, Conflict with Authority Figures, Hierarchy, Prestige, and Social Distance) and emotional exhaustion. Moreover, there was no correlation found between sub-dimensions of Destructive Leadership and Psychological Power Distance.

Using SPSS 27 software, a multiple regression analysis was executed to test the research’s first hypothesis. First of all, by controlling the effects of gender and age, the direct relationship between destructive leadership dimensions and emotional exhaustion was examined. Table  2 displays the analysis findings. The variance explained by emotional exhaustion in this model is R2 = 0.31. The results indicate that Authoritarian leadership (b = 0.29, p  < 0.01) is positively and significantly associated with emotional exhaustion. The study’s first hypothesis is supported by this result. The other sub-dimensions of Destructive leadership (Inadequate leadership skills and unethical behaviors, Inability to deal with new technology and other changes, Nepotism, and Callousness toward subordinates) aren’t associated with emotional exhaustion ( p  > 0.05).

Next, to test hypothesis 2, the moderating effect of psychological power distance sub-dimensions in the relationship between destructive leadership sub-dimensions and emotional exhaustion was examined through SPSS PROCESS 4.1 macro (Hayes, 2018 ). Totally twenty-five regression analyses were conducted. Model 1 of the PROCESS was applied in all analyses, based on 5000 bootstrap samples. As a result of all analyses, it was found that the Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors X Prestige interaction variable (b = 0.17, 0.05 < 95% CI < 0.28), the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes X Hierarchy interaction variable (b = 0,13, 0.02 < 95% CI < 0.24), the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes X Prestige interaction variable (b = 0.16, 0.05 < 95% CI < 0.27), Nepotism X Hierarchy interaction variable (b = 0.10, 0.009 < 95% CI < 0.20) was significantly and positively associated with emotional exhaustion. All the other interactions were not significant.

To understand the interaction of Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors X Prestige interaction, we examined the levels of independent variables based on the levels of moderator variables. To establish low and high values as a default setting, the 16th and 84th percentiles of the moderator variable by PROCESS are taken into consideration (Hayes, 2018 ). In our analysis when the prestige level is low, the association between Inadequate Leadership Skills, Unethical Behaviors, and emotional exhaustion (b = 0.25, 0.11 < 95% CI < 0.39), was relatively low. In contrast, when the prestige was high, Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors were relatively highly related to emotional exhaustion (b = 0.54, 0.40 < 95% CI < 0.67). As the level of prestige increases, Inadequate Leadership Skills, Unethical Behaviors, and emotional exhaustion association also increase. This demonstrates that prestige strengthens the relationship between Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors and Emotional Exhaustion. Along this line, we can say that prestige positively moderates the relationship between Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors, and Emotional Exhaustion. This result validates the study’s second hypothesis.

To figure out the mechanism of the moderating effect of prestige, a simple slope plot was drawn as seen in Fig.  1 . It shows that in the case of a low level of prestige (dashed line) the increase in the Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors leads to a moderately significant difference in emotional exhaustion. However, in the case of a high level of prestige (dashed straight line) differences in the Inadequate Leadership Skills and Unethical Behaviors lead to a relatively higher significant change in emotional exhaustion.

figure 1

Simple slope for inadequate leadership skills and unethical behaviors by Prestige interaction

To understand the interaction of the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes X Hierarchy interaction, we examined the levels of independent variables according to the levels of moderator variables. To establish low and high values as a default setting, the 16th and 84th percentiles of the moderator variable by PROCESS are taken into consideration (Hayes, 2018 ). In our analysis when the hierarchy level is low, the association between Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion (b = 0.28, 0.12 < 95% CI < 0.43), was relatively low. On the contrary, when the hierarchy was high, the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes was relatively highly related to emotional exhaustion (b = 0.49, 0.35 < 95% CI < 0.63). As the level of hierarchy increases, the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion association also increase. This shows that hierarchy strengthens the relationship between the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion. Accordingly, we can say that hierarchy positively moderates the relationship between the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion. This finding supports the second hypothesis of the study.

To figure out the mechanism of the moderating effect of hierarchy, the basic slope plot was created, as seen in Fig.  2 . It shows that in the case of a low level of hierarchy (dashed line) the increase in the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes leads to a moderately significant difference in emotional exhaustion. However, in the case of a high level of hierarchy (dashed straight line) differences in the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes lead to a relatively higher significant change in emotional exhaustion.

figure 2

Simple slope for inability to deal with technology and other changes by Hierarchy interaction

To understand the interaction of the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes X Prestige interaction we examined the levels of independent variables according to the levels of moderator variables. To establish low and high values as a default setting, the 16th and 84th percentiles of the moderator variable by PROCESS are taken into consideration (Hayes, 2018 ). In our analysis when the prestige level is low, the association between the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion (b = 0.27, 0.13 < 95% CI < 0.42), was relatively low. On the contrary, when the prestige was high, the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes was relatively highly related to emotional exhaustion (b = 0.54, 0.39 < 95% CI < 0.68). As the level of prestige increases, the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes, and emotional exhaustion association also increase. This shows that prestige strengthens the relationship between the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion. Accordingly, we can say that prestige positively moderates the relationship between the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes and emotional exhaustion. This finding supports the second hypothesis of the study.

To figure out the mechanism of the moderating effect of prestige, the basic slope plot was created, as seen in Fig.  3 . It shows that in the case of a low level of prestige (dashed line) the increase in Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes leads to a moderately significant difference in emotional exhaustion. However, in the case of a high level of prestige (dashed straight line) differences in Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes lead to a relatively higher significant change in emotional exhaustion.

figure 3

Simple slope for inability to deal with technology and other changes by Prestige interaction

To figure out the mechanism of the moderating effect of the Nepotism X Hierarchy interaction we examined the levels of independent variables according to the levels of moderator variables. To establish low and high values as a default setting, the 16th and 84th percentiles of the moderator variable by PROCESS are taken into consideration (Hayes, 2018 ). In our analysis when the hierarchy level is low, the association between Nepotism and emotional exhaustion (b = 0.21, 0.08 < 95% CI < 0.33), was relatively low. On the contrary, when the hierarchy was high, Nepotism was relatively highly related to emotional exhaustion (b = 0.38, 0.26 < 95% CI < 0.49). As the level of hierarchy increases, Nepotism, and emotional exhaustion association also increase. This shows that hierarchy strengthens the relationship between Nepotism and emotional exhaustion. Accordingly, we can say that hierarchy positively moderates the relationship between Nepotism and emotional exhaustion. This finding supports the second hypothesis of the study.

To figure out the mechanism of the moderating effect of hierarchy, the basic slope plot was drawn as seen in Fig.  4 . It shows that in the case of a low level of hierarchy (dashed line) the increase in Nepotism leads to a moderately significant difference in emotional exhaustion. However, in the case of a high level of hierarchy (dashed straight line) differences in Nepotism lead to a relatively higher significant change in emotional exhaustion.

figure 4

Simple slope for nepotism by Hierarchy interaction

Discussion and conclusion

The study holds some important theoretical implications. The findings of this study offer valuable insights into the moderating role of newly conceptualized Psychological Power Distance (PPD) on the relationship between Destructive Leadership (DL) and Emotional Exhaustion (EE). First, our empirical results support previous research findings regarding the positive correlation between negative leadership styles such as toxic and narcissistic (e.g., Badar et al., 2023 ) and EE. Similar to the findings of Abubakar et al.‘s ( 2017 ) study on nepotism and workplace withdrawal, nepotism, one of the components of DL, was found to be positively correlated ( r  = 0.40, p  <.01) with EE. Hierarchy and nepotism pose risks in different forms (Tytko et al., 2020 ). A great example is the risk of possible downfall when concentrating all power on a small group of privileged relatives and the organs of that group. Also, it explains in terms of the risk of mutual support under hierarchy. When a society is structured in a certain way and has a social structure that allows powerful groups to protect each other’s interests, as is common in Middle Eastern societies, the privilege of that group is guaranteed (Shamaileh & Chaábane, 2022 ). And this mutual reinforcement can be a stable thing over time. Subsequently, the risks and consequences of these dysfunctional and corruptive practices in the context of organizational performance may also be possible.”

On the other hand, we can discuss our empirical results based on the concept of Perceived external prestige (Kamasak & Bulutlar, 2008 ) and social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 2004 ), which are also shaped around the concepts of social value and status. “According to the literature, perceived external prestige, unlike corporate image or corporate reputation, is based on employees’ beliefs (Šulentić et al., 2017 ). Within this framework, when we look at the items measuring the prestige dimension of PPD, we encounter statements that gaining respect and status is important for employees to exert social influence (Cheng et al., 2013 ). Previous studies have shown that the concept of prestige/status is also associated with EE (Sessions et al., 2022 ) and narcissism which is one of the main characteristics of DL (e.g., Cheng et al., 2010 ; Haertel et al., 2023 ; Zeigler et al., 2019 ). Correlationally, our empirical findings also show that prestige also strengthens the relationship between inadequate leadership skills, unethical behaviors, and EE. Besides, This study also created new information about the moderating role of some specific sub-dimensions of PPD in the relationship between DL and EE. Furthermore, the prestige aspect of PPD amplifies the influence of deficient leadership abilities and unethical conduct on EE.

On the other hand, “hierarchies may produce undesirable or dysfunctional consequences” in organizations (Magee & Galinsky, 2008 ; Leavitt, 2005 ). In this context, the fact that differences in Nepotism lead to a relatively higher significant change in EE in case the hierarchy is high can be considered as an empirical finding that may lead to undesirable and dysfunctional results in the organizational context. Additionally, in the context of this research, a specific mediating link of prestige/status seeking emerged in the effect of destructive leadership style on EE. Although this finding is limited, it can be generalized to Middle Eastern countries (e.g., Syria, Lebanon, Iraq) where power distance is high. Therefore, The study holds some managerial implications. These results may assist organizations and leadership training experts in developing interventions to reduce employee EE and abusive supervisory behaviors. In addition, the interactional effect of high levels of hierarchy on the relationship between DL and EE also sheds light on the cultural and psychological processes in societies where PPD is high.

When the prestige was high, the Inability to Deal with New Technology and Other Changes was relatively highly related to EE (b = 0.54, 0.39 < 95% CI < 0.68). In other words, as the level of prestige increases, the challenges of inability to adopt new technology, and EE association also increase. This shows that prestige strengthens the relationship between the challenges of the inability to adopt new technology and EE. Most of the time, it is seen that introducing new technology to the workplace may create fear in the employees. Techniques such as training, communication, and support should be used by the management to minimize these challenges (Ivanov et al., 2020 ). In this context, power distance measures the extent to which subordinates accept control from their leaders or supervisors, or the extent of freedom that they can practice their own beliefs, values, and behaviors (Guzman & Fu, 2022 ). In high power distance cultures, subordinates are not allowed to raise their voice to their supervisor- no matter right or wrong they think. Traditionalists think that this rigid stratification may stop the implementation of new technology developed in high power distance countries like China or Malaysia (e.g., Rithmire, 2023 ). In conclusion, our empirical findings point out a well-established path for future study which focuses on the impact of power distance theories on organizational behavior like technology adaptation.

In a recent research by Harms and his colleagues, they found that the negative effects of DL on EE (Hattab et al. 2022 ) could be reduced by PPD. This means that when subordinates do not perceive a large power distance between them and their leaders, the harmful effect of DL on EE may be minimized. This may be because when subordinates have low PPD from their leaders, they are likely to question the necessity of coping with EE and evaluate demands made by the leaders. On the contrary, when subordinates perceive a large psychological power distance, they are more likely to accept the situation and engage in EE because it is a reflection of decorum or cultural practices. Moreover, it has been suggested that different cultures have their norms about what is good or bad leadership, and these norms can be reflected in the perception of PPD. Investigating the role of power distance in employees’ perception of leaders and better understanding the impact of leaders on employee well-being, will not only inform practices for workplace health intervention but also enlighten leadership researchers in discussing the universal and contingency theory of leadership.

The study of the moderating role of PPD will be advantageous for the following reasons. Firstly, fostering a transparent and merit-based work culture in high PPD countries is a way to reduce and prevent nepotism in the employment setting (Kirya, 2020 ). Secondly by identifying how and under what circumstances different types of leaders may impact differently on employees, human resource practitioners will have better insight into leader selection and training. For example, if an organization operates in a relatively low power distance culture such as the United States (Hofstede, 2001 ), then the findings from our research suggest that both supportive and directive leadership styles may be effective in reducing employees’ EE. However, if the same organization aims to expand to countries with higher power distance, especially those in the Middle East, it will be beneficial to have leaders with less directive and more supportive leadership styles in charge. In this circumstance, human resource practitioners can use the cultural dimensions of different societies such as power distance to evaluate the suitability of leaders’ approaches and make necessary modifications.

Limitations and future research directions

Because most concepts in the social sciences are interrelated in some way, theoretical frameworks that provide a holistic perspective are particularly important in facilitating research.

Therefore, in order not to expand the scope of the research too much, thereby the theoretical structure is limited to specific correlated variables. On the other hand, organizations are rich in complex interpersonal interactions, and these relational dynamics, combined with unique organizational factors, can differentiate the relationship of DL and EE from one organization to another. Besides, culture is another important antecedent predictor in terms of organizational outcomes. For example, the culture that affects the follower’s questioning, ethical decision-making, or tolerance of the unethical behavior of the supervisor (Cohen, 1995 ). Therefore, it should not be ignored that the level of EE may differ depending on the level of exposure to DL along with culture, situational circumstances, level of motivation, or personality characteristics of employees.

The PPD is a newly conceptualized notion. In this context, we also believe that the hypothesis and the results of our study may lead to new research questions for unexplored fields. In addition, there are several practical implications within our research. We explained the organic mechanism between DL and EE under the moderation of PPD. In this sense, employees and practitioners need to understand the motivation and psychological contract (Rousseau, 1990 ) level of employees under the reign of DL. On the other hand, based on our conceptual elaboration in this paper, we also suggest future research on gender, because, male and female forms of destructive leadership can differ significantly in terms of gender barriers and catalysts such as role fit. Finally the complex nature of the research variables, qualitative research is needed to explore which individual resources (e.g., self-efficacy) or behaviors will be negatively affected to cope with destructive leaders. As an additional comment, the significant effect of hierarchy on emotional exhaustion in the Turkish sample, where power distance is high (Hofstede, 2001 ) could be a starting point for future research in the context of the impact of cultural homogenization/globalization via internal uniformity (Conversi, 2014 ).

This study has some limitations. First of all, this study is conducted in a rather small region in Türkiye. Examining this research model at the international level in different provinces and different cultures will make strong contributions to the literature. The second is the investigation of the proposed causal relationship through the application of a cross-sectional research methodology. Thirdly, because our study relies on self-reported questionnaires, it is susceptible to common method biases and social desirability.

Abubakar, A. M., Namin, B. H., Harazneh, I., Arasli, H., & Tunç, T. (2017). Does gender moderates the relationship between favoritism/nepotism, supervisor incivility, cynicism and workplace withdrawal: A neural network and SEM approach. Tourism Management Perspectives , 23 , 129–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tmp.2017.06.001

Article   Google Scholar  

Adamovic, M. (2023). Breaking down power distance into 5 dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences , 208 , 112178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2023.112178

Ahmad, A. R., ALHAMMADI, A. H. Y., & Jameel, A. S. (2021). National culture, leadership styles and job satisfaction: An empirical study in the United Arab Emirates. The Journal of Asian Finance Economics and Business , 8 (6), 1111–1120. https://doi.org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no6.1111

Aïssaoui, R., & Fabian, F. (2015). The French paradox: Implications for variations in global convergence. Journal of International Management , 21 (1), 31–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intman.2014.12.004

Aydinay, M., Cakici, A., & Cakici, A. (2022). The effect of destructive leadership on self-efficacy and counterproductive work behaviors: A research on service sector employees in Mersin, Turkey. Journal of Global Business Insights, 6 (2), 186-206. https://www.doi.org/10.5038/2640-6489.6.2.1166

Badar, K., Aboramadan, M., & Plimmer, G. (2023). Despotic vs narcissistic leadership: Differences in their relationship to emotional exhaustion and turnover intentions. International Journal of Conflict Management , 34 (4), 818–837. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCMA-12-2022-0210

Baig, S. A., Iqbal, S., Abrar, M., Baig, I. A., Amjad, F., Zia-ur-Rehman, M., & Awan, M. U. (2021). Impact of leadership styles on employees’ performance with moderating role of positive psychological capital. Total Quality Management & Business Excellence , 32 (9–10), 1085–1105. https://doi.org/10.1080/14783363.2019.1665011

Bakker, A. B., & Costa, P. L. (2014). Chronic job burnout and daily functioning: A theoretical analysis. Burnout Research , 1 (3), 112–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.burn.2014.04.003

Bianchi, M., Giacomini, E., Crinelli, R., Radici, L., Carloni, E., & Magnani, M. (2015). Dynamic transcription of ubiquitin genes under basal and stressful conditions and new insights into the multiple UBC transcript variants. Gene, 573(1), 100-109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gene.2015.07.030

Bono, J. E., Foldes, H. J., Vinson, G., & Muros, J. P. (2007). Workplace emotions: The role of supervision and leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92 (5), 1357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1357

Brislin, R., Lonner, W., & Thorndike, R. (1973). Cross-cultural Research methods . John Wiley.

Google Scholar  

Brouwers, M., & Paltu, A. (2020). Toxic leadership: Effects on job satisfaction, commitment, turnover intention and organisational culture within the South African manufacturing industry. SA Journal of Human Resource Management , 18 (1), 1–11. https://hdl.handle.net/10520/EJC-1fa457614d

BurnsJr, W. A. (2021). A typology of destructive leadership: Pseudotransformational, laissez- faire, and unethical causal factors and predictors. Destructive Leadership and Management Hypocrisy: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 49–66). Emerald Publishing Limited.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Byrne, A., Dionisi, A. M., Barling, J., Akers, A., Robertson, J., Lys, R., & Dupre, K. (2014). The depleted leader: The influence of leaders’ diminished psychological resources on leadership behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly , 25 , 344–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.09.003

Cable, D. M., & Edwards, J. R. (2004). Complementary and supplementary fit: a theoretical and empirical integration. Journal of applied psychology, 89(5), 822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.5.822

Carl, D., Gupta, V., & Javidan, M. (2004). Power distance. Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of, 62(2004), 513-563.

Cascio, W. F., & Aguinis, H. (2008). Research in industrial and organizational psychology from 1963 to 2007: Changes, choices, and trends. Journal of Applied Psychology , 93 (5), 1062–1081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.1062

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Chang, M. L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of teacher burnout: Examining the emotional work of teachers. Educational Psychology Review , 21 , 193–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-009-9106-y

Chen, X., & Cheung, Y. W. (2020). School characteristics, strain, and adolescent delinquency: A test of macro-level strain theory in China. Asian Journal of Criminology , 15 (1), 65–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11417-019-09296-x

Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior , 31 (5), 334–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.004

Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 104 (1), 103. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030398

Chi, S. C. S., & Liang, S. G. (2013). When do subordinates’ emotion-regulation strategies matter? Abusive supervision, subordinates’ emotional exhaustion, and work withdrawal. The Leadership Quarterly , 24 (1), 125–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.08.006

Chiaburu, D. S., Chakrabarty, S., Wang, J., & Li, N. (2015). Organizational support and citizenship behaviors: A comparative crosscultural meta-analysis. Management International Review , 55 (5), 707–736. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11575-015-0253-8

Cohen, D. V. (1995). Creating ethical work climates: A socioeconomic perspective. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 24(2), 317-343. https://doi.org/10.1016/1053-5357(95)90024-1

Conversi, D. (2014). Between the hammer of globalization and the anvil of nationalism: Is Europe’s complex diversity under threat? Ethnicities , 14 (1), 25–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468796813487727

Cordes, C. L., & Dougherty, T. W. (1993). A review and integration of research on job burnout. Academy of Management Review , 18 , 621–656. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1993.9402210153

d’Iribarne, P. (1996). The usefulness of an ethnographic approach to the international comparison of organizations. International Studies of Management & Organization , 26 (4), 30–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/00208825.1996.11656693

De Clercq, D., Fatima, T., & Jahanzeb, S. (2021). Ingratiating with despotic leaders to gain status: The role of power distance orientation and self-enhancement motive. Journal of Business Ethics , 171 (1), 157–174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-019-04368-5

Ding, J., Qiao, P., Wang, J., & Huang, H. (2022). Impact of food safety supervision efficiency on preventing and controlling mass public crisis. Front Public Health , 10 (1052273). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.105227

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Dorfman, P., & Howell, J. P. (1988). Dimensions of national culture and effectiveleadership patterns: Hofstede revisited. In R. N. Farmer, & E. G. McGoun (Eds.),Advances in international comparative management. London, England: JAI Press, 172-150.

Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S., & Skogstad, A. (2007). Destructive leadership behaviour: A definition and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly , 18 (3), 207–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2007.03.002

Ergin, C. (1992). Doktor ve hemşirelerde tükenmişlik ve Maslach Tükenmişlik Ölçeğinin uyarlanması, VII . Ulusal Psikoloji Kongresi, Hacettepe Üniversitesi, Ankara.

Farh, J. L., Hackett, R. D., & Liang, J. (2007). Individual-level cultural values as moderators of perceived organizational support–employee outcome relationships in China: Comparing the effects of power distance and traditionality. Academy of management journal, 50(3), 715-729. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2007.25530866

Fors Brandebo, M. (2020). Destructive leadership in crisis management. Leadership & Organization Development Journal , 41 (4), 567–580. https://doi.org/10.1108/LODJ-02-2019-0089

Fors Brandebo, M., Nilsson, S., & Larsson, G. (2016). Leadership: Is bad stronger than good? Leadership & Organization Development Journal , 37 (6), 690–710. https://doi.org/10.1108/LODJ-09-2014-0191

Gkorezis, P., Petridou, E., & Krouklidou, T. (2015). The detrimental effect of machiavellian leadership on employees’ emotional exhaustion: Organizational cynicism as a mediator. Europe’s Journal of Psychology , 11 (4), 619. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i4.988

Glasø, L., & Vie, T. L. (2009). Toxic emotions at work. Scandinavian Journal of Organizational Psychology , 2 , 13–16.

Gonzalez, N. L., (2021). The impact of culture on business negotiations. Honors Projects . 839. https://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/honorsprojects/839

Gooty, J., Connelly, S., Griffith, J., & Gupta, A. (2010). Leadership, affect and emotions: A state of the science review. The Leadership Quarterly, 21(6), 979-1004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.005

Guzman, F. A., & Fu, X. (2022). Leader–subordinate congruence in power distance values and voice behaviour: A person–supervisor fit approach. Applied Psychology , 71 (1), 271–295. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12320

Haertel, T. M., Leckelt, M., Grosz, M. P., Kuefner, A. C., Geukes, K., & Back, M. D. (2023). Pathways from narcissism to leadership emergence in social groups. European Journal of Personality , 37 (1), 72–94. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070211046266

Hattab, S., Wirawan, H., Salam, R., Daswati, D., & Niswaty, R. (2022). The effect of toxic leadership on turnover intention and counterproductive work behaviour in Indonesia public organisations. International Journal of Public Sector Management , 35 (3), 317–333. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJPSM-06-2021-0142

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based perspective (2nd ed.). The Guilford.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations . Martino Publishing.

Book   Google Scholar  

Hetrick, A. L., Mitchell, M. S., Villarosa-Hurlocker, M. C., & Sullivan, T. S. (2022). The consequence of unethical leader behavior to Employee Well-Being: Does support f rom the Organization mitigate or exacerbate the stress experience? Human Performance , 35 (5), 323–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2022.2123486

Hobfoll, S. E. (2011). Conservation of resources theory: Its implication for stress, health, and resilience. The Oxford Handbook of Stress Health and Coping , 127 , 147.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values, abridged version . Sage Publications.

Hofstede, G. (1991). Empirical models of cultural differences. In N. Bleichrodt & P. J. D. Drenth (Eds.), Contemporary issues in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 4–20). Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.

Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions andorganizations across nations . Sage Publications.

Hsieh, C. W., Jin, M. H., & Guy, M. E. (2011). Consequences of work-related emotions: Analysis of a cross-section of public service workers. The American Review of Public Administration , 42 (1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0275074010396078

Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and political change in 43 societies . Princeton University Press.

Ivanov, S., Kuyumdzhiev, M., & Webster, C. (2020). Automation fears: Drivers and solutions. Technology in Society , 63 , 101431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techsoc.2020.101431

Jiao, Y. C., & Wang, Y. C. (2023). Under the mask: The double-edged sword effect of leader self-sacrifice on employee work outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology , 14 , 1052623.

Jonason, P. K., Czerwiński, S. K., Tobaldo, F., Ramos-Diaz, J., Adamovic, M., Adams, B. G., & Sedikides, C. (2022). Milieu effects on the Dark Triad traits and their sex differences in 49 countries. Personality and Individual Differences , 197 , 111796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111796

Kamasak, R., & Bulutlar, F. (2008). The impact of communication climate and job satisfaction in employees’ external Prestige perceptions. Yönetim Ve Ekonomi , 18 (2), 133–144.

Kelemen, T. K., Matthews, S. H., & Breevaart, K. (2020). Leading day-to-day: A review of the daily causes and consequences of leadership behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly , 31 (1), 101344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2019.101344

Khakhar, P., & Rammal, H. G. (2013). Culture and business networks: International business negotiations with Arab managers. International Business Review, 22(3), 578-590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibusrev.2012.08.002

Kirya, M. T. (2020). Promoting anti-corruption, transparency and accountability in the recruitment and promotion of health workers to safeguard health outcomes. Global Health Action , 13 (sup1), 1701326. https://doi.org/10.1080/16549716.2019.1701326

Koç, O., Bozkurt, S., Taşdemir, D. D., & Günsel, A. (2022). The moderating role of intrinsic motivation on the relationship between toxic leadership and emotional exhaustion. Frontiers in Psychology , 13 , 1047834. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1047834

Kong, M., & Jogaratnam, G. (2007). The influence of culture on perceptions of service employee behavior. Managing Service Quality: An International Journal , 17 (3), 275–297. https://doi.org/10.1108/09604520710744308

Korkmazyurek, Y., & Korkmazyurek, H. (2023). The gravity of culture on project citizenship behaviors. Current Psychoogyl , 42 , 27415–27427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03849-7

Krasikova, D. V., Green, S. G., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Destructive leadership: A theoretical review, integration, and future research agenda. Journal of Management , 39 (5), 1308–1338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206312471388

Krumov, K., Negruti, A., Hristova, P., & Krumova, A. (2016). Perceptions of toxic leaders- empirical research. J Innov Entrep Sustain Dev , 1 , 3–17.

Lam, C. K., Walter, F., & Huang, X. (2017). Supervisors’ emotional exhaustion and abusive supervision: The moderating roles of perceived subordinate performance and supervisor self-monitoring. Journal of Organizational Behavior , 38 (8), 1151–1166. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2193

Lam, S. S., Schaubroeck, J., & Aryee, S. (2002). Relationship between organizational justice and employee work outcomes: a cross‐national study. Journal of organizational behavior, 23(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.131

Larsson, G., Fors Brandebo, M., & Nilsson, S. (2012). Destrudo-L: Development of a short scale designed to measure destructive leadership behaviours in a military context. Leadership & Organization Development Journal , 33 (4), 383–400.

Leavitt, H. J. (2005). Top down: Why hierarchies are here to stay and how to manage them more effectively . (No Title).

Leonidou, L. C., Aykol, B., Larimo, J., Kyrgidou, L., & Christodoulides, P. (2021). Enhancing international buyer-seller relationship quality and long-term orientation using emotional intelligence: The moderating role of foreign culture. Management International Review , 61 (3), 365–402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11575-021-00447-w

Li, W., Lu, Y., Makino, S., & Lau, C. M. (2017). National power distance, status incongruence, and CEO dismissal. Journal of World Business , 52 (6), 809–818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2017.08.001

Liao, Z., Lee, H. W., Johnson, R. E., Song, Z., & Liu, Y. (2021). Seeing from a short-term perspective: When and why daily abusive supervisor behavior yields functional and dysfunctional consequences. Journal of Applied Psychology , 106 (3), 377. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000508

Lindholm, C. (2008). The Islamic middle east: tradition and change . John Wiley & Sons.

Loi, R., Lai, J. Y., & Lam, L. W. (2012). Working under a committed boss: A test of the relationship between supervisors’ and subordinates’ affective commitment. The Leadership Quarterly , 23 (3), 466–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.12.001

Mackey, J. D., Ellen III, B. P., McAllister, C. P., & Alexander, K. C. (2021). The dark side of leadership: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis of destructive leadership research. Journal of Business Research, 132, 705-718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.10.037

Mackey, J. D., McAllister, C. P., Maher, L. P., & Wang, G. (2019). Leaders and followers behaving badly: A meta-analytic examination of curvilinear relationships between destructive leadership and followers’ workplace behaviors. Personnel Psychology , 72 (1), 3–47.

Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). 8 social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. The Academy of Management Annals , 2 (1), 351–398. https://doi.org/10.1080/19416520802211628

Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., & Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory manual (3rd ed.). Consulting Psychologists Press.

Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology , 52 (1), 397–422. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397

Matta, S., Rogova, N., & Luna-Cortés, G. (2022). Investigating tolerance of uncertainty, COVID-19 concern, and compliance with recommended behavior in four countries: The moderating role of mindfulness, trust in scientists, and power distance. Personality and Individual Differences , 186 , 111352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111352

Maznevski, M. L., Gomez, C. B., DiStefano, J. J., Noorderhaven, N. G., & Wu, P. C. (2002). Cultural dimensions at the individual level of analysis: The cultural orientations framework. International journal of cross cultural management, 2(3), 275-295. https://doi.org/10.1177/147059580223001

Meydan, C. H., Basim, H. N., & BASAR, U. (2014). Power distance as a moderator of the relationship between organizational citizenship behavior and impression management. Eurasian Journal of Business and Economics , 7 (13), 105–118.

Moon, T. W., & Hur, W. M. (2011). Emotional intelligence, emotional exhaustion, and job performance. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal , 39 (8), 1087–1096. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2011.39.8.1087

Murad, M., Jiatong, W., Shahzad, F., & Syed, N. (2021). The influence of despotic leadership on counterproductive work behavior among police personnel: Role of emotional exhaustion and organizational cynicism. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology , 36 (3), 603–615. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-021-09470-x

Naseer, S., & Raja, U. (2021). Why does workplace bullying affect victims’ job strain? Perceived organization support and emotional dissonance as resource depletion mechanisms. Current Psychology , 40 (9), 4311–4323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00375-x

Obi, I. M. O., Bollen, K., Aaldering, H., Robijn, W., & Euwema, M. C. (2020). Servant leadership, third-party behavior, and emotional exhaustion of followers. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research . https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12184

Ojo, O. (2012). Influence of organizational culture on employee work behavior. International Journal of Contemporary Business Studies , 3 (11), 46–57.

Peng, J., Li, M., Wang, Z., & Lin, Y. (2021). Transformational leadership and employees’ reactions to organizational change: Evidence from a meta-analysis. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science , 57 (3), 369–397. https://doi.org/10.1177/0021886320920366

Pillay, J., & April, K. (2022). Developing Leadership Maturity: Ego Development and Personality Coaching. Effective Executive , 25 (1), 40–73.

Qiu, P., Yan, L., Zhang, Q., Guo, S., Liu, C., Liu, H., & Chen, X. (2023). Organizational display rules in nursing: Impacts on caring behaviors and emotional exhaustion through emotional labor. International Nursing Review . https://doi.org/10.1111/inr.12876

Rao, A. N., & Pearce, J. L. (2016). Should management practice adapt to cultural values? The evidence against power distance adaptation. Cross Cultural & Strategic Management , 23 (2), 1–49. https://doi.org/10.1108/CCSM-03-2014-0035

Rasid, S. Z. A., Manaf, M. A. A., & Quoquab, F. (2013). Leadership and organizational commitment in the islamic banking context: The role of organizational culture as a mediator. American Journal of Economics , 3 (5), 171–176. https://doi.org/10.5923/c.economics.201301.29

Rattrie, L. T., Kittler, M. G., & Paul, K. I. (2020). Culture, burnout, and engagement: A meta- analysis on national cultural values as moderators in JD‐R theory. Applied Psychology , 69 (1), 176–220. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12209

Rithmire, M. (2023). Precarious ties: Business and the state in authoritarian Asia . Oxford University Press.

Rosenstein, A. (2017). Disruptive and unprofessional behaviors. In K. Brower, & M. Riba (Eds.), Physician Mental Health and Well-Being. Integrating Psychiatry and Primary Care . Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55583-6_3

Rousseau, D. M. (1990). New hire perceptions of their own and their employer’s obligations: A study of psychological contracts. Journal of Organizational Behavior , 11 (5), 389–400. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030110506

Rumjaun, A., & Narod, F. (2020). Social Learning Theory—Albert Bandura. Science education in theory and practice: An introductory guide to learning theory (pp. 85–99)

Ryan, P., Odhiambo, G., & Wilson, R. (2021). Destructive leadership in education: A transdisciplinary critical analysis of contemporary literature. International Journal of Leadership in Education , 24 (1), 57–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/13603124.2019.1640892

Salas-Vallina, A., Alegre, J., & López‐Cabrales, Á. (2021). The challenge of increasing employees’ well‐being and performance: How human resource management practices and engaging leadership work together toward reaching this goal. Human Resource Management , 60 (3), 333–347. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.22021

Schilling, J., & Schyns, B. (2015). The causes and consequences of bad leadership. Zeitschrift für Psychologie . https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000185

Schmid, E. A., Verdorfer, P., A., & Peus, C. (2019). Shedding light on leaders’ self- interest: Theory and measurement of exploitative leadership. Journal of Management , 45 (4), 1401–1433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206317707810

Schwartz, S. H. (2014). National culture as value orientations: Consequences of value differences and cultural distance. In Handbook of the Economics of Art and Culture (Vol. 2, pp. 547-586). Elsevier.

Sessions, H., Nahrgang, J. D., Baer, M. D., & Welsh, D. T. (2022). From zero to hero and back to zero: The consequences of status inconsistency between the work roles of multiple jobholders. Journal of Applied Psychology , 107 (8), 1369. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000935

Shahzad, K., Iqbal, R., Nauman, S., Shahzadi, R., & Luqman, A. (2023). How a despotic project manager jeopardizes Project Success: The role of Project Team Members’ emotional exhaustion and Emotional Intelligence. Project Management Journal , 54 (2), 194–208. https://doi.org/10.1177/87569728221145891

Shamaileh, A., & Chaábane, Y. (2022). Institutional favoritism, income, and political trust: Evidence from Jordan. Comparative Politics , 54 (4), 741–764. https://doi.org/10.5129/001041522X16331935725154

Shore, B., & Cross, B. J. (2005). Exploring the role of national culture in the management of large scale international science projects. International Journal of Project Management, 23 , 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2004.05.009

Strack, J., Lopes, P. N., & Esteves, F. (2015). Will you thrive under pressure or burn out? Linking anxiety motivation and emotional exhaustion. Cognition and Emotion , 29 (4), 578–591. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.922934

Šulentić, T. S., Žnidar, K., & Pavičić, J. (2017). The key determinants of perceived external prestige (PEP)–qualitative research approach. Management: Journal of Contemporary Management Issues , 22 (1), 49.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. Political psychology (pp. 276–293). Psychology Press.

Tang, G., Chen, Y., van Knippenberg, D., & Yu, B. (2020). Antecedents and consequences of empowering leadership: Leader power distance, leader perception of team capability, and team innovation. Journal of Organizational Behavior , 41 (6), 551–566. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2449

Taras, V. (2014). Catalogue of instruments for measuring culture . https://www.researchgate.net/publication/266520890_Catalogue_of_Instruments_for_Measuring_Culture

Tavakol, M., & Wetzel, A. (2020). Factor Analysis: a means for theory and instrument development in support of construct validity. International Journal of Medical Education, 6 (11), 245–247. https://doi.org/10.5116/ijme.5f96.0f4a . PMID: 33170146; PMCID: PMC7883798.

Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of Management Journal , 43 (2), 178–190. https://doi.org/10.5465/1556375

Tepper, B. J. (2007). Abusive supervision in work organizations: Review, synthesis, and research agenda. Journal of Management , 33 (3), 261–289. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206307300812

Tepper, B. J., Simon, L., & Park, H. M. (2017). Abusive supervision. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior , 4 , 123–152. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-041015-062539

Tytko, A., Smokovych, M., Dorokhina, Y., Chernezhenko, O., & Stremenovskyi, S. (2020). Nepotism, favoritism and cronyism as a source of conflict of interest: Corruption or not? Amazonia Investiga , 9 (29), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.34069/AI/2020.29.05.19

Veldsman, T. H. (2012). The growing cancer endangering organisations: Toxic leadership. Conference on Leadership in Emerging Countries presented by Department of Industrial Psychology and People Management, Faculty of Management, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg.

Wang, M., Sinclair, R., & Deese, M. N. (2010). Understanding the causes of destructive leadership behavior: A dualprocess model. In B. Schyns, & T. Hansbrough (Eds.), When leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes, and ethical failures (pp. 73–97). Information Age.

Weigl, M., Beck, J., Wehler, M., & Schneider, A. (2017). Workflow interruptions and stress at work: a mixed-methods study among physicians and nurses of a multidisciplinary emergency department. BMJ open, 7(12), e019074. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-019074

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review , 92 (4), 548–573.

Yang, J. S. (2020). Differential moderating effects of collectivistic and power distance orientations on the effectiveness of work motivators. Management Decision , 58 (4), 644–665. https://doi.org/10.1108/MD-10-2018-1119

Yazıcıoğlu, Y., & Erdoğan, S. (2004). SPSS applied scientific research methods . Detay Publishing.

Zeigler-Hill, V., Vrabel, J. K., McCabe, G. A., Cosby, C. A., Traeder, C. K., Hobbs, K. A., & Southard, A. C. (2019). Narcissism and the pursuit of status. Journal of Personality , 87 (2), 310–327. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12392

Zhang, Y., & Liao, Z. (2015). Consequences of abusive supervision: A meta-analytic review. Asia Pacific Journal of Management , 32 , 959–987. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10490-015-9425-0

Download references

Open access funding provided by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Türkiye (TÜBİTAK).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Toros University, Mersin, Turkey

Yavuz Korkmazyurek & Metin Ocak

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yavuz Korkmazyurek .

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Korkmazyurek, Y., Ocak, M. The moderating role of psychological power distance on the relationship between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion. Curr Psychol (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06016-2

Download citation

Accepted : 15 April 2024

Published : 14 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-06016-2

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

APS

New Content From Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science

  • Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science
  • Cognitive Dissonance
  • Meta-Analysis
  • Methodology
  • Preregistration
  • Reproducibility

research article social psychology

A Practical Guide to Conversation Research: How to Study What People Say to Each Other Michael Yeomans, F. Katelynn Boland, Hanne Collins, Nicole Abi-Esber, and Alison Wood Brooks  

Conversation—a verbal interaction between two or more people—is a complex, pervasive, and consequential human behavior. Conversations have been studied across many academic disciplines. However, advances in recording and analysis techniques over the last decade have allowed researchers to more directly and precisely examine conversations in natural contexts and at a larger scale than ever before, and these advances open new paths to understand humanity and the social world. Existing reviews of text analysis and conversation research have focused on text generated by a single author (e.g., product reviews, news articles, and public speeches) and thus leave open questions about the unique challenges presented by interactive conversation data (i.e., dialogue). In this article, we suggest approaches to overcome common challenges in the workflow of conversation science, including recording and transcribing conversations, structuring data (to merge turn-level and speaker-level data sets), extracting and aggregating linguistic features, estimating effects, and sharing data. This practical guide is meant to shed light on current best practices and empower more researchers to study conversations more directly—to expand the community of conversation scholars and contribute to a greater cumulative scientific understanding of the social world. 

Open-Science Guidance for Qualitative Research: An Empirically Validated Approach for De-Identifying Sensitive Narrative Data Rebecca Campbell, McKenzie Javorka, Jasmine Engleton, Kathryn Fishwick, Katie Gregory, and Rachael Goodman-Williams  

The open-science movement seeks to make research more transparent and accessible. To that end, researchers are increasingly expected to share de-identified data with other scholars for review, reanalysis, and reuse. In psychology, open-science practices have been explored primarily within the context of quantitative data, but demands to share qualitative data are becoming more prevalent. Narrative data are far more challenging to de-identify fully, and because qualitative methods are often used in studies with marginalized, minoritized, and/or traumatized populations, data sharing may pose substantial risks for participants if their information can be later reidentified. To date, there has been little guidance in the literature on how to de-identify qualitative data. To address this gap, we developed a methodological framework for remediating sensitive narrative data. This multiphase process is modeled on common qualitative-coding strategies. The first phase includes consultations with diverse stakeholders and sources to understand reidentifiability risks and data-sharing concerns. The second phase outlines an iterative process for recognizing potentially identifiable information and constructing individualized remediation strategies through group review and consensus. The third phase includes multiple strategies for assessing the validity of the de-identification analyses (i.e., whether the remediated transcripts adequately protect participants’ privacy). We applied this framework to a set of 32 qualitative interviews with sexual-assault survivors. We provide case examples of how blurring and redaction techniques can be used to protect names, dates, locations, trauma histories, help-seeking experiences, and other information about dyadic interactions. 

Impossible Hypotheses and Effect-Size Limits Wijnand van Tilburg and Lennert van Tilburg

Psychological science is moving toward further specification of effect sizes when formulating hypotheses, performing power analyses, and considering the relevance of findings. This development has sparked an appreciation for the wider context in which such effect sizes are found because the importance assigned to specific sizes may vary from situation to situation. We add to this development a crucial but in psychology hitherto underappreciated contingency: There are mathematical limits to the magnitudes that population effect sizes can take within the common multivariate context in which psychology is situated, and these limits can be far more restrictive than typically assumed. The implication is that some hypothesized or preregistered effect sizes may be impossible. At the same time, these restrictions offer a way of statistically triangulating the plausible range of unknown effect sizes. We explain the reason for the existence of these limits, illustrate how to identify them, and offer recommendations and tools for improving hypothesized effect sizes by exploiting the broader multivariate context in which they occur. 

research article social psychology

It’s All About Timing: Exploring Different Temporal Resolutions for Analyzing Digital-Phenotyping Data Anna Langener, Gert Stulp, Nicholas Jacobson, Andrea Costanzo, Raj Jagesar, Martien Kas, and Laura Bringmann  

The use of smartphones and wearable sensors to passively collect data on behavior has great potential for better understanding psychological well-being and mental disorders with minimal burden. However, there are important methodological challenges that may hinder the widespread adoption of these passive measures. A crucial one is the issue of timescale: The chosen temporal resolution for summarizing and analyzing the data may affect how results are interpreted. Despite its importance, the choice of temporal resolution is rarely justified. In this study, we aim to improve current standards for analyzing digital-phenotyping data by addressing the time-related decisions faced by researchers. For illustrative purposes, we use data from 10 students whose behavior (e.g., GPS, app usage) was recorded for 28 days through the Behapp application on their mobile phones. In parallel, the participants actively answered questionnaires on their phones about their mood several times a day. We provide a walk-through on how to study different timescales by doing individualized correlation analyses and random-forest prediction models. By doing so, we demonstrate how choosing different resolutions can lead to different conclusions. Therefore, we propose conducting a multiverse analysis to investigate the consequences of choosing different temporal resolutions. This will improve current standards for analyzing digital-phenotyping data and may help combat the replications crisis caused in part by researchers making implicit decisions. 

Calculating Repeated-Measures Meta-Analytic Effects for Continuous Outcomes: A Tutorial on Pretest–Posttest-Controlled Designs David R. Skvarc, Matthew Fuller-Tyszkiewicz  

Meta-analysis is a statistical technique that combines the results of multiple studies to arrive at a more robust and reliable estimate of an overall effect or estimate of the true effect. Within the context of experimental study designs, standard meta-analyses generally use between-groups differences at a single time point. This approach fails to adequately account for preexisting differences that are likely to threaten causal inference. Meta-analyses that take into account the repeated-measures nature of these data are uncommon, and so this article serves as an instructive methodology for increasing the precision of meta-analyses by attempting to estimate the repeated-measures effect sizes, with particular focus on contexts with two time points and two groups (a between-groups pretest–posttest design)—a common scenario for clinical trials and experiments. In this article, we summarize the concept of a between-groups pretest–posttest meta-analysis and its applications. We then explain the basic steps involved in conducting this meta-analysis, including the extraction of data and several alternative approaches for the calculation of effect sizes. We also highlight the importance of considering the presence of within-subjects correlations when conducting this form of meta-analysis.   

Reliability and Feasibility of Linear Mixed Models in Fully Crossed Experimental Designs Michele Scandola, Emmanuele Tidoni  

The use of linear mixed models (LMMs) is increasing in psychology and neuroscience research In this article, we focus on the implementation of LMMs in fully crossed experimental designs. A key aspect of LMMs is choosing a random-effects structure according to the experimental needs. To date, opposite suggestions are present in the literature, spanning from keeping all random effects (maximal models), which produces several singularity and convergence issues, to removing random effects until the best fit is found, with the risk of inflating Type I error (reduced models). However, defining the random structure to fit a nonsingular and convergent model is not straightforward. Moreover, the lack of a standard approach may lead the researcher to make decisions that potentially inflate Type I errors. After reviewing LMMs, we introduce a step-by-step approach to avoid convergence and singularity issues and control for Type I error inflation during model reduction of fully crossed experimental designs. Specifically, we propose the use of complex random intercepts (CRIs) when maximal models are overparametrized. CRIs are multiple random intercepts that represent the residual variance of categorical fixed effects within a given grouping factor. We validated CRIs and the proposed procedure by extensive simulations and a real-case application. We demonstrate that CRIs can produce reliable results and require less computational resources. Moreover, we outline a few criteria and recommendations on how and when scholars should reduce overparametrized models. Overall, the proposed procedure provides clear solutions to avoid overinflated results using LMMs in psychology and neuroscience.   

Understanding Meta-Analysis Through Data Simulation With Applications to Power Analysis Filippo Gambarota, Gianmarco Altoè  

Meta-analysis is a powerful tool to combine evidence from existing literature. Despite several introductory and advanced materials about organizing, conducting, and reporting a meta-analysis, to our knowledge, there are no introductive materials about simulating the most common meta-analysis models. Data simulation is essential for developing and validating new statistical models and procedures. Furthermore, data simulation is a powerful educational tool for understanding a statistical method. In this tutorial, we show how to simulate equal-effects, random-effects, and metaregression models and illustrate how to estimate statistical power. Simulations for multilevel and multivariate models are available in the Supplemental Material available online. All materials associated with this article can be accessed on OSF ( https://osf.io/54djn/ ).   

Feedback on this article? Email  [email protected]  or login to comment.

APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines .

Please login with your APS account to comment.

Privacy Overview

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Word frequency and cognitive effort in turns-at-talk: turn structure affects processing load in natural conversation.

Christoph Rühlemann

  • 1 University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
  • 2 Leibniz Institute for the German Language (IDS), Mannheim, Baden-Württemberg, Germany

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

Select one of your emails

You have multiple emails registered with Frontiers:

Notify me on publication

Please enter your email address:

If you already have an account, please login

You don't have a Frontiers account ? You can register here

Frequency distributions are known to widely affect psycholinguistic processes. The effects of word frequency in turns-at-talk, the nucleus of social action in conversation, have, by contrast, been largely neglected. This study probes into this gap by applying corpus-linguistic methods on the conversational component of the British National Corpus (BNC) and the Freiburg Multimodal Interaction Corpus (FreMIC). The latter includes continuous pupil size measures of participants of the recorded conversations, allowing for a systematic investigation of patterns in the contained speech and language on the one hand and their relation to concurrent processing costs they may incur in speakers and recipients on the other hand. We test a first hypothesis in this vein, analyzing whether word frequency distributions within turns-at-talk are correlated with interlocutors' processing effort during the production and reception of these turns. Turns are found to generally show a regular distribution pattern of word frequency, with highly frequent words in turn-initial positions, mid-range frequency words in turn-medial positions, and low-frequency words in turn-final positions. Speakers' pupil size is found to tend to increase during the course of a turn at talk, reaching a climax towards the turn end. Notably, the observed decrease in word frequency within turns is inversely correlated with the observed increase in pupil size in speakers, but not in recipients, with steeper decreases in word frequency going along with steeper increases in pupil size in speakers. We discuss the implications of these findings for theories of speech processing, turn structure, and information packaging. Crucially, we propose that the intensification of processing effort in speakers during a turn at talk is owed to an informational climax, which entails a progression from highfrequency, low-information words through intermediate levels to low-frequency, high-information words. At least in English conversation, interlocutors seem to make use of this pattern as one way to achieve efficiency in conversational interaction, creating a regularly recurring distribution of processing load across speaking turns, which aids smooth turn transitions, content prediction, and effective information transfer.

Keywords: conversation, processing, Pupillometry, Word frequencies, Corpora

Received: 18 Apr 2023; Accepted: 13 May 2024.

Copyright: © 2024 Rühlemann and Barthel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Christoph Rühlemann, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

  • Open access
  • Published: 10 May 2024

Digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for violent radicalization among young Canadians: a latent profile analysis

  • Diana Miconi 1 ,
  • Tara Santavicca 2 ,
  • Rochelle L. Frounfelker 3 ,
  • Aoudou Njingouo Mounchingam 4 &
  • Cécile Rousseau 5  

BMC Psychology volume  12 , Article number:  260 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

75 Accesses

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

Despite the prominent role that digital media play in the lives and mental health of young people as well as in violent radicalization (VR) processes, empirical research aimed to investigate the association between Internet use, depressive symptoms and support for VR among young people is scant. We adopt a person-centered approach to investigate patterns of digital media use and their association with depressive symptoms and support for VR.

A sample of 2,324 Canadian young people (M age = 30.10; SD age = 5.44 ; 59% women) responded to an online questionnaire. We used latent profile analysis to identify patterns of digital media use and linear regression to estimate the associations between class membership, depressive symptoms and support for VR.

We identified four classes of individuals with regards to digital media use, named

Average Internet Use/Institutional trust, Average internet use/Undifferentiated Trust, Limited Internet Use/Low Trust and Online Relational and Political Engagement/Social Media Trust. Linear regression indicated that individuals in the Online Relational and Political Engagement/Social Media Trust and Average Internet Use/Institutional trust profiles reported the highest and lowest scores of both depression and support for VR, respectively.

Conclusions

It is essential to tailor prevention and intervention efforts to mitigate risks of VR to the specific needs and experiences of different groups in society, within a socio-ecological perspective. Prevention should consider both strengths and risks of digital media use and simulteaneously target both online and offline experiences and networks, with a focus on the sociopolitical and relational/emotional components of Internet use.

Peer Review reports

The recent increase in support for – and direct engagement in – ideologically motivated violence among youth can be associated with the increase in social polarization in society [ 1 ] as well as the specificities of adolescence and early adulthood, a seminal period for the development of ideologies [ 2 , 3 ]. Violent radicalization (VR) is a complex and multidimensional phenomenon [ 4 ] defined as a process whereby an individual or a group increases support for violence as a legitimate means to reach a specific (e.g., political, social, religious) goal [ 5 ]. Noteably, VR processes are increasingly occurring online [ 6 , 7 ]. Internet use has been primarily investigated in the field of terrorism studies and with samples of radicalized individuals [ 6 , 8 ]. Less is known about the association of digital media use, social polarization and attitudes towards support for VR among young people. Although the association between attitudes and behaviors is not a linear one, positive attitudes towards VR can contribute to the creation of socially polarized environments that fuel conflicts and shatter social solidarities, resulting in some cases in extremist ideologies and the normalization of violence. In such contexts, vulnerable individuals - such as those experiencing significant social grievances - are at higher risk of engaging in violent acts and extremism. Thus, in a primary prevention perspective, a reduction in support of VR among youth can result in an overall decrease of violence in our societies in the short and long-term [ 9 , 10 , 11 ].

Although numerous interventions target online literacy and social media use as potential ways to counter violent extremism [ 7 ], empirical research on their effectiveness is scarce and the role that Internet use plays in the development of positive attitudes towards VR among young people is largely understudied. While depressive symptomatology, which has also been increasing among young people in the past decade [ 12 , 13 ], is associated with both digital media use and support for VR [ 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 ], empirical research has not yet examined the associations between these variables simultaneously in one study. The current study aims to fill this gap in the literature by empirically investigating if and how patterns of digital media use are differently associated with depressive symptoms and support for VR among a sample of Canadian young people via a person-centered approach. Given the prominent role that digital media use play in both VR processes and mental health among young people, a better understanding of risk and protective factors associated with digital media use is warranted to inform and tailor evidence-based prevention programs that could significantly help reduce social ruptures and the associated risk of violence.

Digital media use and support for VR

The online space has become a central developmental context for young people [ 18 , 19 ]. Empirical evidence remains mixed, suggesting that digital media use can be either a risk or protective factor across multiple developmental outcomes depending on a complex interplay between both online and offline factors [ 18 ]. A consensus is now emerging that the specific behaviors in which youth engage online, rather than overall digital media per se, are key determinants of well-being. Yet, gaps in knowledge remain [ 20 ].

On the one hand, digital media can be used to connect with peers and to counter isolation, thus extending or reinforcing one’s social support network and possibly one’s trust in institutions and in democracy. On the other hand, the Internet can provide instant and unfiltered access to content and groups that propagate fake news, extreme beliefs and encourage violent actions, representing one of the main settings that can facilitate disaffiliation phenomena and recruitment of young people by extremist groups [ 7 , 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 ]. Notably, whereas the majority of young people go online, only a minority of them get involved in VR processes. As such, it is likely that digital media use does not have a linear relationship with support for VR, but that specific constellations of digital media use are differentially associated with support for VR [ 8 , 25 ].

Young people’s use of digital media is complex and heterogeneous [ 18 ], making the measurement and conceptualization of digital media use a challenging area [ 26 , 27 ]. In this study, we focus on some aspects of digital media use that have been theoretically and/or empirically associated with VR, namely time on social media, reasons for Internet use (work, informational, entertainment, social), news literacy, trust in specific online sources of information (news, peers, influencers, government, youtube), preference for online social interactions and online political interactions.

The Internet can be used for multiple purposes, spanning from work or entertainment, to relational maintenance and social interaction [ 18 , 28 ]. Although spending more time online has been associated with increased exposure to extremist content [ 23 ], whether this exposure is associated with risks of VR is yet unclear [ 29 ]. Overall, the impact of time spent on social media on a variety of social and health outcomes including VR varies based on the specific online activities and experiences [ 8 , 18 , 20 ].

Of importance, the Internet is currently the most important source of information for young people [ 30 , 31 ], but trust on the validity of information from official governmental websites as well as from social media (e.g., Instagram, Twitter, Youtube) can vary between individuals. Misinformation and beliefs in conspiracy theories have been associated with higher support for VR [ 32 , 33 ]. News literacy is considered a potential avenue to countering both misinformation, social polarization and online extremism [ 34 , 35 ]. News literacy is defined as the ability to find/identify/recognize news, critically evaluate and produce them [ 36 ]. However, empirical research that examines the association between news literacy and support for VR is lacking.

Prior research has found that preference for online social interactions over face-to-face relationships represents a risk factor for support for VR [ 37 , 38 ]. Preference for online social interactions is characterized by beliefs that one is safer, more confident, more comfortable and appreciated when online as opposed to offline [ 39 ] and is considered a component of problematic Internet use as it implies problematic relational experiences offline.

Some studies suggest that actively seeking and engaging with extremist content online is associated with higher risk of VR [ 8 , 22 , 25 ]. Although online interactions with strangers have been associated with higher risk of psychological distress [ 17 , 40 ], the extent to which interactions with known and unknown people around political or current issues are associated, if at all, with support for VR has yet to be explored [ 23 ].

Given the variety in online experiences and type of digital media use, a person-centered approach via a latent profile analysis (LPA) facilitates examining different constellations of digital media use among young adults and associations between latent groups and support for VR. As VR is the result of complex and unique interplays between personal and social/contextual variables [ 4 , 41 , 42 ], identifying patterns of vulnerabilities online via a person-centered approach can inform the development of tailored VR prevention programs targeting digital media use.

The present study

The present study adopts a person-centered approach to investigate: 1) patterns of digital media use among young Canadians. Specifically we focus on reasons for digital media use (i.e., work, entertainment, socialization, information), reported trust in different sources of online information (i.e., official government and news websites and social media), news literacy, time on social media, preference for online social interactions and online political interactions (e.g., posting/discussing with peers vs strangers, having conflicts online about these issues); 2) the association between patterns of digital media use and levels of depressive symptoms; and 3) the association between patterns of digital media use and support for VR. We expect to identify at least two groups of young people who differ in their reported digital media use. Given that we do not have a priori knowledge of the class structure in the data, we did not have a priori hypotheses about the association between each profile and depressive symptoms. However, we anticipate that the group(s) that will report the highest levels of depressive symptoms will also be at higher risk of supporting VR.

Participants

A total of 2,695 participants answered an online survey; missing outcome data ( n =362) and individuals identifying as “other” gender ( n =9) were removed for methodological concerns given the very small sample size of this gender group. Final sample size was 2,324 participants (59.3% women; mean age = 30.10; SD = 5.44 ). Socio-demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1 .

Data were collected in November 2021, during the COVID-19 pandemic in Alberta, Ontario, and Quebec. Participants were recruited through the Leger360 online platform with over 500,0000 registered members and answered the survey in either English or French [ 43 ]. informed consent to participate was obtained electronically from all of the participants in the study, and response rate was 53.8%. Exclusion criteria were individuals under the age of 18 or above 41. Study protocol and procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of.

Support for VR

The Radicalism Intention Scale (RIS) is a 4-item subscale of the Activism and Radicalism Intention Scales (ARIS) [ 44 ]. It assesses an individual’s readiness to participate in illegal and violent behavior in the name of one’s group or organization. Respondents rated their agreement with four statements on a seven-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more support for VR (range 4-28). The scale has good psychometric properties among young adults [ 45 ] (α = .89; Ω = 0.89).

Time spent on social media (daily)

Participants were asked to identify how many hours they spend on social media on a typical day (i.e., less than 2 hours, 2-4 hours, 4-6 hours, and 6 hours or more).

Reasons for Internet use

Four statements on Internet use were presented (i.e., using Internet: for personal relationships, to actively search for information/news, for entertainment, and for work). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale how much they used the Internet for each reason (not at all, a little, moderately, a lot, most of the time).

News literacy

Was measured as a subscale of the literacy scale by Jones-Jang et al. [ 36 ]. Participants were asked using a 5-point Likert scale how much they agreed with each statement (six items, from 1-strongly disagree to 5- strongly agree, range 6 - 30)(α = .80; Ω = 0.80).

Trust on online sources of information

Five statements around trusting different sources of online information were presented, namely trust in news, peers, influencers, government, and YouTube sources of information. Participants were asked to indicate how often they trust each source of information on a 4-point Likert scale (never, rarely, sometimes, often).

Preference for online social interactions

(PFOSI) was measured with the 13-item Social Comfort subscale of the Online Cognition Scale [ 46 ]. Participants rated on a 8-point Likert scale (range 0 – 91) how much they agreed with statements describing their relationships with people who they know primarily through the Internet (e.g., chat rooms, message boards, online gaming communities). Higher scores indicate more preference for online social interactions (α = .92; Ω = 0.92).

Online political interactions

Participants were asked to indicate on a 6-point Likert scale (from “None/No time at all”, to “Several times a day”) how often their online interactions were oriented around these four statements: posted information about politics/current affairs on social media, discussed politics/current affairs with people you know, discussed politics/current affairs with people you do not know, had verbal conflicts with known people around information shared/posted online.

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms were measured by using the 15-item subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) [ 47 ]. Items are rated on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) based on how much discomfort that problem has caused them during the past seven days, and a total score is obtained by computing the mean of all items. The clinical cut-off is set at 1.75 (score range from 1 to 4) and scores have been recoded as below (0) or above (1) this cut-off. The HSCL-25’s psychometric qualities have been well established [ 48 ] (α = .94; Ω = 0.94).

Socio-demographic variables

Participants provided information on their age, gender (man or woman), education (None/Less than high school, High school graduate, Apprenticeship, technical institute, trade or vocational school, College, CEGEP or other non-university certificate or diploma or University certificate, diploma or degree), Income ($19,999 or less, $20,000- $39,999, $40,000- $59,999, $60,000 - $79,999, $80,000- $99,999, $100,000 or more), employment (not employed, employed -essential, employed – non-essential), generational status (first-generation immigrant, second-generation immigrant, and third generation or more immigrant/non-immigrant), province (Alberta, Ontario, Quebec), religious beliefs (no religion, religion), and age.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were conducted using R software [ 49 ]. Missing data were imputed using the Random Forest method via the mice package [ 50 , 51 ]. Sensitivity analysis suggested that missing data and multiple imputations did not alter the observed patterns of associations. First, we estimated the LPA model around variables related to digital media use via the tidyLPA package [ 52 ]. LPA is an analytic strategy that attempts to identify subgroups of people within a heterogeneous population who has a high degree of homogeneity in responses on a set of indicators. The appropriate number of latent profiles was selected based on the Akaike information criterion (AIC), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), the Sample-size-adjusted BIC (SABIC), Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT), characteristics of the profiles (interpretability of response profiles or uniqueness) and a conservative profile sample size (>10%) [ 53 , 54 , 55 , 56 ]. Lower AIC, BIC and SABIC values and a statistically significant BLRT indicate a better model fit [ 53 , 54 ]. Once the best LPA solution was identified, the level of entropy (acceptable if >.70) and Average Posterior Class Probability (AvePP; acceptable if >.70) were examined to determine the accuracy of classification [ 57 ].

Next, based on the predicted probabilities of profile membership made by the LPA, we assigned each participant to a specific profile. Analyses were then conducted on the univariable associations between sociodemographic characteristics and profile membership. Frequencies of profile membership by sociodemographic characteristics can be found in Table 3 .

Lastly, we conducted linear regression analyses that estimated support for VR as a function of profile membership. A sequential model building approach was used to evaluate the associations between profiles and support for VR. Model 1 presents the unadjusted association between profile and support for VR; model 2 adjusts for sociodemographic characteristics, and model 3 adjusts for sociodemographic characteristics and depression.

Latent Profile Structure

LPA models 2 through 6 are presented in Table 2 along associated BIC and log-likelihood values. The four-class solution was selected as the best fit for sample size of profiles (>10%) and interpretability of findings, despite not having the lowest BIC value. Figure 1 presents profile membership item response probabilities for digital media use. Participants in all profiles had a high probability of reporting average levels of news literacy. Unique class characteristics emerged around time spent on social media and overall Internet use preference for online social interactions, online political interactions and trusting multiple sources of information. Profile 1, named Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust is characterized by individuals who demonstrated average Internet use yet infrequently used the Internet for interactions around politics/current affairs and showed undifferentiated trust towards information found on-line, regardless of the source. Participants in Profile 2, named Limited Internet use/Low Trust , infrequently used the Internet across the considered reasons and reported a low probability of trusting news and government sources compared to information from social media (e.g., peers, influencers, youtube). Profile 3, named Average Internet use/Institutional trust , is characterized by participants with average and undifferentiated internet use, who were more likely to report greater trust in institutional sources of online information (e.g., news, government) compared to other social media sources. Profile 4, named Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust , consists of individuals with a high probability of preferring online, as opposed to in person, social interactions and spending a large amount of time on social media on a daily basis. In addition, participants in Profile 4 had a high probability of using the Internet for discussing politics and other issues with both peers and strangers, actively posting on-line about politics, and were more likely to report conflicts online compared to all other profiles. Profile 4 participants had a lower probability of trusting news and government sources compared to other sources of information online (peers, influencers, youtube). Overall, Profile 1 and 3 included 46.8% and 27.9% of participants, respectively. Profile 2 was smaller and included 11.4% of participants, while Profile 4 included 13.9% of participants.

figure 1

Four-Profile Solution with Standardized Mean by Item Responses ( N = 2324)

Note. PFOSI Preference for online social interactions

Profile belonging, sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms

Table 3 represents sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms by profile for study participants. All variables were significantly associated ( p < 0.05) with profile belonging at the univariable level. The Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile included a higher representation of women, non-immigrant, employed and non-religious participants, as well as participants who reported high education and income. A total of 45% of participants in this profile scored above the clinical cut-off for depressive symptoms. Participants in the Limited Internet use/Low Trust profile had a higher probability of being less educated, reporting a lower income and more unemployement. Participants in this profile were more likely to live in Alberta. Profile 3, Average Internet use/Institutional trust included participants who were highly educated, had high income, without an immigration background (third generation or more) and without a religion. Participants in this profile reported also the lowest levels of depression (37.5% above clinical cut-off) and more of them lived in Quebec. Finally, the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile had an overrepresentation of men, immigrants, participants with a religion and who lived mainly in Ontario. In addition, participants in this group were overall educated but reported low income and high unemployment. A total of 70% of participants in this profile scored above the clinical cut-off to our measure of depressive symptoms.

Associations of profile membership with support for VR

Profile membership was associated with scores on the RIS ( p < 0.001). Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile were more likely to report higher levels of support for VR compared to the other profiles in both unadjusted and adjusted models. Specifically, belonging to this profile was associated with a 0.91 ( SE = 0.06, p < 0.001) increase in support for VR compared to the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile when controlling for sociodemographic variables and depressive symptoms (Table 4 ). Belonging to the Average Internet use/Institutional trust profile was associated with a -0.267 ( SE = 0.046, p < 0.001) decrease in support for VR compared to the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile when controlling for sociodemographic variables and depression (Table 4 ). Gender, generation, province, age, and depressive symptoms were also associated with support for VR ( p < 0.05). Men, first generation immigrants, participants from Ontario, younger participants, and participants reporting more depressive symptoms were more likely to report higher support for VR. Religion, income and education were not significantly associated with support for VR (Table 4 ).

The current study investigated patterns of digital media use in a sample of young adults from three Canadian provinces. In addition, we examined whether these patterns were differentially associated with depressive symptoms and support for VR. Four profiles emerged from our LPA, confirming the pertinence of using a person-centered approach to shed light on the complex patterns of digital media use among young people. Overall, profiles differentiated participants mostly in terms of trust on specific sources of information and level and type of online engagement.

The two largest profiles ( Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust and Average Internet use/Insitutional trust) differed primarily in their trust of online sources of information. Specifically, individuals in the Average Internet use/Insitutional trust profile reported to trust more frequently institutional sources of information (i.e., government and news) rather than social media (i.e., youtube, influencers, and peers), suggesting an overall acceptance of mainstream information and of the status quo. In contrast, the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust group showed average levels of trust to all sources of information alike. This group spent slightly more time online than the Average Internet use/Insitutional trust one , but overall these two groups did not differ much in their online social or political interactions. These two groups included 74.7% of participants, indicating a divide in the population mostly linked to what online sources to trust for information. The remaining participants were equally distributed between the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust and the Low Internet use/Low trust profiles. Participants in both of these profiles trusted more frequently alternative social media sources of information compared to institutional ones, but they differed in overall levels of trust, with the Limited Internet use/Low trust group reporting overall low levels of trust, especially for institutional sources of information. Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile reported high levels of trust in alternative social media sources of information and were more actively and politically engaged online with both peers and especially with strangers. They spent more time online and preferred online social interactions more compared to the other profiles. Taken together, these findings suggest that patterns of digital media use echo the increasing polarization in our societies [ 58 , 59 ] around issues of trust/distrust, engagement/disengagement as well as a variety of negative/positive online experiences. Indeed, the most important variables to differentiate the four profiles were related to the frequency of trusting different online sources of information as well as specific social and political interactions online, rather than reasons for Internet use or news literacy, which on the contrary did not seem to play a significant role in determining profile membership.

We suggest that the divide around trust in online information and engagement needs to be situated in the broader socio-political context, which can partly explain the socio-demographic differences we found across profiles.The Average Internet use/Insitutional trust and the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profiles consisted of more affluent and more educated participants, mostly employed and without an immigration background. Participants in these profiles may benefit from more privileges in society, which can favor their trust in mainstream institutional sources of information online [ 60 , 61 , 62 ] . Indeed, participants in the Average Internet use/Institutional trust group were more likely to report the highest levels of education and income as well as the lowest levels of depressive symptoms followed by the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile. The difference in levels of depression between these two profiles can also be associated with the presence of younger participants and more women in the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile compared to the Average internet use/Institutional trust one. The Low Internet use/Low trust and Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profiles included more participants reporting lower income. Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust group included a higher percentage of men, participants with an immigration background and professing a religion – although participants in this profile reported an education level similar to the two larger profiles. This profile reported concerning levels of depression (70.2% above clinical cut-off). Relying on the internet for relational and political purposes combined with more frequent trust in alternative social media sources of information and less privileges in society can jeopardize young people’s mental health. Within a socio-ecological perspective, the fact that this profile is made up of primarily educated men with an immigrant background may represent a form of double-bind in which some groups may feel alienated because official discourses and stances about equity in Canada are contradicted by daily life experiences. This group’s pattern of digital media use may be related to the hardships, grievances and social deprivation experienced by minorities both online and offline. The combination of negative life experiences with high emotional distress may lead to experience overall negative and conflictual online social and political exchanges, subsequently legitimazing violence as an ultimate solution [ 16 , 17 , 63 ]. Besides reporting low income similarly to the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust group, the Limited Internet use/Low trust profile included less educated and more unemployed participants compared to all other profiles, mostly without an immigration background. Participants in this group may not be content with their socio-political reality, and disengage from social and political issues, at least online. Noteworthy, our profiles suggest that digital media use is closely intertwined with social experiences offline. Interventions should consider this complex interaction and adopt a socio-ecological approach to both research and intervention, tailored not only to the different groups in society but also addressing the gap between them to mend the social fabric.

With regards to depressive symptoms and support for VR, the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust reported the highest levels of depression and support for VR, followed by the Limited Internet use/Low trust profile. The fact that the two groups that reported less trust in institutional sources of information compared to alternative social media showed more depressive symptoms and support for VR indicates that issues of trust are important to address with young people in prevention and intervention efforts. Given that individuals in these groups had overall a lower status in society, compared to the other two profiles, it is possible that they may have been experiencing more social deprivation and grievances during the pandemic and have been more sensitive to the anti-system rhetoric which provided meaning to this perceived injustice [ 60 ]. This divide aligns with the emergence of polarized social movements in the whole of Canada (e.g., pro- and anti-vaxx groups during the pandemic). Promoting a sense of agency and belonging as well as ensuring that young people can express their opinions and have a purpose in life may help decrease depressive symptoms and reduce overall socio-political distrust and disengement both online and offline, which can in turn contribute to reduce the legitimation of violence. However, such interventions need to consider the social adversity and deprivation experienced by young people and be tailored to the specific needs and challenges that they face. Multi-level systemic interventions that target online and offline socio-political macro-determinants of mental health and injustices in our societies are needed above and beyond individual intervention programs.

The association between membership to the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile and support for VR aligns with prior studies pointing to an association between active online political engagement and interactions and support for VR [ 8 , 23 , 35 ]. Noteworthy, this was a characteristic that clearly distinguished the Online active political engagement/Social media trust profile from all other profiles. Online relational and political engagement should be addressed in prevention and intervention, while also addressing possible isolation and injustices experienced offline. The association between membership to the Limited Internet use/Low trust and support for VR can be related to an overall distrust in society and especially in government and official institutions, which has been found to represent a risk factor for VR [ 32 ].

As expected, the group that was at higher risk of supporting VR was also the one that reported the highest level of depressive symptoms, which were significantly and positively associated with support for VR, confirming prior evidence [ 38 , 64 , 65 , 66 , 67 ]. Depressive symptoms do not necessarily lead to greater risk of VR [ 68 ]. Yet, multiple studies indicate a positive association between depressive symptoms and support for VR [ 38 , 64 , 65 , 66 , 67 ]. Although directionality of associations remain to be established, available evidence suggests that youth who interact more with strangers online [ 17 , 40 ], who prefer online social interactions [ 69 , 70 , 71 ] and who experience more social adversity [ 14 , 67 ] are at higher risk of depression, which can partly explain the higher scores of depressive symptoms found among the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profiles . Identifying as a man and being younger were also risk factors for support for VR, in line with prior studies [ 7 , 15 ], underlining the pertinence for future studies to focus on young people and to consider specificities by gender in VR studies [ 14 , 29 , 32 , 45 ].

Limitations

This study has several limitations. Most importantly, the cross-sectional design prevents us from drawing any conclusions about causality. Longitudinal studies are needed to shed light on the trajectories of associations between patterns of digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for VR. Second, our study is based on a convenience sample with a relatively high socio-economic level and education. This means that our results may not be generalizable to a larger, general population of young adults. Nonetheless, our online method of recruitment is appropriate given the sensitivity of the topic and the challenges of conducting research during a pandemic. Third, all data are based on young people’s self-reports and social desirability biases cannot be excluded. Fourth, our measures of digital media use were limited and not comprehensive of the broad range of possible online experiences. Given the rapidly evolving and dynamic aspects of the Internet, the availability of validated measures for different facets of Internet use remains a challenge for future studies. Last, our data were collected during the COVID-19 pandemic in three Canadian provinces, and results cannot be easily generalized to other provinces or countries, nor to a non-pandemic context.

Despite these limitations, our findings suggest that digital media use, psychological distress and their interaction play a role in processes of VR among young people and need to be situated and understood within a socio-ecological and social justice perspective. Specifically, trust in different sources of information and social and political experiences online are as relevant as the emotional and relational experiences of young people. The dynamic associations among these key elements have to be considered simultanously when reflecting on VR prevention and digital media use among young people. Prevention efforts should be adapted to the needs of specific populations and consider the diversity of their online/offline experiences. Indeed, our results suggests that online experiences are intertwined with offline experiences in society, in particular with grievances, and that an attention to the rapidly evolving socio-political scenario is warranted when designing intervention programs to prevent processes of VR among young people targeting their digital media use. The fact that self-reported news literacy did not differ across profiles questions the pertinence of VR prevention programs that target mainly news literacy skills among youth. Our findings support preliminary results that showed that media literacy did not protect youth from exposure to extremist content online [ 35 ] or risks of VR [ 25 ]. It has been argued that programs aimed to foster digital literacy may be associated with improved technical competence but leave participants “critically naïve” [ 72 ], failing to situate digital competence within the broader socio-political context. Although digital literacies may still be relevant skills to promote among young people, our findings suggest that, when it comes to the prevention of VR processes, critical thinking skills, supportive environments and a social justice approach to intervention may be equally important.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Venkatesh V, Rousseau C, Morin D, Hassan G. Violence as collateral damage of the COVID-19 pandemic. The Conversation. 2021. https://theconversation.com/collateral-damage-of-covid-19-rising-rates-of-domestic-and-social-violence-143345 .

Harpviken AN. Psychological vulnerabilities and extremism among western youth: A literature review. Adolescent Research Review. 2020;5(1):1–26.

Article   Google Scholar  

Steinberg LD. Age of opportunity: Lessons from the new science of adolescence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 2014.

McGilloway A, Ghosh P, Bhui K. A systematic review of pathways to and processes associated with radicalization and extremism amongst Muslims in Western societies. International review of psychiatry. 2015;27(1):39–50.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Schmid AP. Radicalisation, de-radicalisation, counter-radicalisation: A conceptual discussion and literature review. International Centre for Counter-terrorism (ICCT). Research Paper. 2013. https://www.icct.nl/sites/default/files/import/publication/ICCT-Schmid-Radicalisation-De-Radicalisation-Counter-Radicalisation-March-2013_2.pdf .

Oppetit A, Campelo N, Bouzar L, Pellerin H, Hefez S, Bronsard G, et al. Do radicalized minors have different social and psychological profiles from radicalized adults? Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2019;10:644.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Amit S, Kafy AA. A systematic literature review on preventing violent extremism. J Adolesc. 2022;94(8):1068–80.

Hassan G, Brouillette-Alarie S, Alava S, Frau-Meigs D, Lavoie L, Fetiu A, et al. Exposure to extremist online content could lead to violent radicalization: A systematic review of empirical evidence. International Journal of Developmental Science. 2018(Preprint):1-18.

Eisenman DP, Flavahan L. Canaries in the coal mine: Interpersonal violence, gang violence, and violent extremism through a public health prevention lens. International review of psychiatry. 2017;29(4):341–9.

Weine S, Eisenman DP, Kinsler J, Glik DC, Polutnik C. Addressing violent extremism as public health policy and practice. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression. 2017;9(3):208–21.

Adam-Troian J, Tecmen A, Kaya A. Youth extremism as a response to global threats? European Psychologist. 2021;26(1):15–28.

Knapstad M, Sivertsen B, Knudsen AK, Smith ORF, Aarø LE, Lønning KJ, et al. Trends in self-reported psychological distress among college and university students from 2010 to 2018. Psychological medicine. 2021;51(3):470–8.

Lipson SK, Phillips MV, Winquist N, Eisenberg D, Lattie EG. Mental health conditions among community college students: A national study of prevalence and use of treatment services. Psychiatric services. 2021:appi. ps. 202000437.

Miconi D, Levinsson A, Frounfelker RL, Li ZY, Oulhote Y, Rousseau C. Cumulative and independent effects of experiences of social adversity on support for violent radicalization during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of depression. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2022;57:1–13.

Rousseau C, Miconi D, Frounfelker RL, Hassan G, Oulhote Y. A repeated cross-sectional study of sympathy for violent radicalization in Canadian college students. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2020;90(4):406–18.

Baker DA, Algorta GP. The relationship between online social networking and depression: A systematic review of quantitative studies. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19(11):638–48.

Ybarra ML, Alexander C, Mitchell KJ. Depressive symptomatology, youth Internet use, and online interactions: A national survey. Journal of adolescent health. 2005;36(1):9–18.

Stavropoulos V, Motti-Stefanidi F, Griffiths MD. Risks and opportunities for youth in the digital era: A cyber-developmental approach to mental health. European Psychologist. 2022;27(2):86–101.

Subrahmanyam K, Šmahel D. Connecting online behavior to adolescent development: A theoretical framework. Digital youth: Springer; 2011. p. 27–39.

Google Scholar  

Hamilton L, Gross B. How Has the Pandemic Affected Students’ Social-Emotional Well-Being? A Review of the Evidence to Date: Center on Reinventing Public Education; 2021.

Kaakinen M, Keipi T, Räsänen P, Oksanen A. Cybercrime victimization and subjective well-being: An examination of the buffering effect hypothesis among adolescents and young adults. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2018;21(2):129–37.

Turner N, Holt TJ, Brewer R, Cale J, Goldsmith A. Exploring the relationship between opportunity and self-control in youth exposure to and sharing of online hate content. Terrorism and Political Violence. 2022;35(7):1–16.

Costello M, Barrett-Fox R, Bernatzky C, Hawdon J, Mendes K. Predictors of viewing online extremism among America’s youth. Youth & Society. 2020;52(5):710–27.

Morris E. Children: extremism and online radicalization. Journal of Children and Media. 2016;10(4):508–14.

Wolfowicz M, Hasisi B, Weisburd D. What are the effects of different elements of media on radicalization outcomes? A systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2022;18(2):e1244.

V Shah NK, R. Lance Holbert, Dhavan. Connecting and disconnecting with civic life: Patterns of Internet use and the production of social capital. Political Commun. 2001;18(2):141-62.

Lee N-J, Shah DV, McLeod JM. Processes of political socialization: A communication mediation approach to youth civic engagement. Communication Research. 2013;40(5):669–97.

Ekström M, Olsson T, Shehata A. Spaces for public orientation? Longitudinal effects of Internet use in adolescence. Information, Communication & Society. 2014;17(2):168–83.

Conway M. Determining the role of the internet in violent extremism and terrorism: Six suggestions for progressing research. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism. 2017;40(1):77–98.

Anderson M, Jiang J. Teens, Social Media, and Technology 2018. Pew Research Center; 2018.

Ohme J. Updating citizenship? The effects of digital media use on citizenship understanding and political participation. Information, Communication & Society. 2019;22(13):1903–28.

Levinsson A, Miconi D, Li Z, Frounfelker RL, Rousseau C. Conspiracy theories, psychological distress, and sympathy for violent radicalization in young adults during the CoViD-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2021;18(15):7846.

Vv Mulukom, Pummerer LJ, Alper S, Bai H, Čavojová V, Farias J, et al.  Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. 2022;301:114912.

Henschke A, Reed A. Toward an ethical framework for countering extremist propaganda online. Studies in Conflict and Terrorism. 2021:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/1057610X.2020.1866744 .

Schmuck D, Fawzi N, Reinemann C, Riesmeyer C. Social media use and political cynicism among German youth: the role of information-orientation, exposure to extremist content, and online media literacy. Journal of Children and Media. 2022;16(3):313–31.

Jones-Jang SM, Mortensen T, Liu J. Does media literacy help identification of fake news? Information literacy helps, but other literacies don’t. American Behavioral Scientist. 2021;65(2):371–88.

Ellis H, Miller E, Sideridis G, Frounfelker RL, Miconi D, Abdi S, et al. Risk and protective factors associated with attitudes in support of violent radicalization: variations by geographic location. Int J Public Health. 2021;66:617053.

Miconi D, Geenen G, Frounfelker RL, Levinsson A, Rousseau C. Meaning in life, future orientation and support for violent radicalization among Canadian college students during the CoViD-19 pandemic. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:765908.

Caplan SE. Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: development of a theory-based cognitive–behavioral measurement instrument. Computers in human behavior. 2002;18(5):553–75.

Gross EF, Juvonen J, Gable SL. Internet use and well-being in adolescence. Journal of social issues. 2002;58(1):75–90.

Miconi D, Rousseau C. Another way out: A positive youth development (PYD) approach to the study of violent radicalizaton in Quebec (Canada). In: Dimitrova R, Wiium N, editors. Handbook of positive youth development: Advancing research, policy, and practice in global contexts. New York: Springer; 2021. p. 415–29.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Gøtzsche-Astrup O, Van den Bos K, Hogg MA. Radicalization and violent extremism: Perspectives from research on group processes and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2020;23(8):1127–36.

Leger 360. Leger Marketing 2022 [Available from: https://leger360.com/ .

Moskalenko S, McCauley C. Measuring political mobilization: The distinction between activism and radicalism. Terrorism and political violence. 2009;21(2):239–60.

Frounfelker RL, Frissen T, Miconi D, Lawson J, Brennan RT, d’Haenens L, et al. Transnational evaluation of the Sympathy for Violent Radicalization Scale: Measuring population attitudes toward violent radicalization in two countries. Transcult Psychiatry. 2021;58(5):669–82.

Davis RA, Flett GL, Besser A. Validation of a new scale for measuring problematic Internet use: Implications for pre-employment screening. Cyberpsychology & behavior. 2002;5(4):331–45.

Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Rickels K, Uhlenhuth EH, Covi L. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): A self-report symptom inventory. Systems Research and Behavioral Science. 1974;19(1):1–15.

Mollica RF, Caspi-Yavin Y, Bollini P, Truong T, Tor S, Lavelle J. The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire: validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1992;180(2):111–6.

R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2017.

van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(3):1–67.

Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009;338:b2393.

Rosenberg JM, Beymer PN, Anderson DJ, Van Lissa C, Schmidt JA. tidyLPA: An R package to easily carry out latent profile analysis (LPA) using open-source or commercial software. J Open Source Software. 2019;3(30):978.

Ferguson SL, G. Moore EW, Hull DM Finding latent groups in observed data A primer on latent profile analysis in Mplus for applied researchers. Int J Behav Devel. 2020;44(5):458-68.

Tein J-Y, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct Equation Model: a multidisciplinary J. 2013;20(4):640–57.

Lubke G, Neale MC. Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: Resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behav Res. 2006;41(4):499–532.

Spurk D, Hirschi A, Wang M, Valero D, Kauffeld S. Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. J Vocational Behav. 2020;120:103445.

Masyn KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In Todd D. Little (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology vol. 2. Oxford Library of Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013. p. 551–611.

Pickup M, Stecula D, Van Der Linden C. Novel coronavirus, old partisanship: COVID-19 attitudes and behaviours in the United States and Canada. Can J Pol Sci/Revue canadienne de science politique. 2020;53(2):357–64.

Boxell L, Gentzkow M, Shapiro JM. Cross-country trends in affective polarization. Rev Econ Stat. 2024;106(2):557–65.

Gibson B, Schneider J, Talamonti D, Forshaw M. The impact of inequality on mental health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review. Can Psychol/Psychologie Canadienne. 2021;62(1):101.

Bornand T, Klein O. Political Trust by Individuals of low Socioeconomic Status: The Key Role of Anomie. Soc Psychol Bull. 2022;17:1–22.

Goubin S, Hooghe M. The Effect of Inequality on the Relation Between Socioeconomic Stratification and Political Trust in Europe. Soc Justice Res. 2020;33(2):219–47.

Vannucci A, Simpson EG, Gagnon S, Ohannessian CM. Social media use and risky behaviors in adolescents: A meta-analysis. J Adolesc. 2020;79:258–74.

Bhui K, Warfa N, Jones E. Is violent radicalisation associated with poverty, migration, poor self-reported health and common mental disorders? PloS one. 2014;9(3):e90718.

Bhui K, Silva MJ, Topciu RA, Jones E. Pathways to sympathies for violent protest and terrorism. Br J Psychiatry J Mental Sci. 2016;209(6):483–90.

Bhui K, Otis M, Silva MJ, Halvorsrud K, Freestone M, Jones E. Extremism and common mental illness: Cross-sectional community survey of White British and Pakistani men and women living in England. Br J Psychiatry. 2020;217(4):547–54.

Rousseau C, Oulhote Y, Lecompte V, Mekki-Berrada A, Hassan G, El Hage H. Collective identity, social adversity and college student sympathy for violent radicalization. Transcult Psychiatry. 2019;58(5):654–68.

Misiak B, Samochowiec J, Bhui K, Schouler-Ocak M, Demunter H, Kuey L, et al. A systematic review on the relationship between mental health, radicalization and mass violence. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;56:51–9.

Gámez-Guadix M. Depressive symptoms and problematic Internet use among adolescents: Analysis of the longitudinal relationships from the cognitive–behavioral model. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2014;17(11):714–9.

Seabrook EM, Kern ML, Rickard NS. Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Mental Health. 2016;3(4): e5842.

Vannucci A, McCauley Ohannessian C. Social media use subgroups differentially predict psychosocial well-being during early adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(8):1469–93.

Hinrichsen J, Coombs A. The five resources of critical digital literacy: a framework for curriculum integration. Res Lear Technol. 2014;21(0):1–16.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Our work is funded by a Digital Citizen Contribution Program grant awarded to DM by Canadian Heritage and by a project grant awarded to CR by the Canadian Institute of Health Research (CIHR).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Educational Psychology and Adult Education, University of Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada

Diana Miconi

MUHC Research Institute, Montréal, QC, Canada

Tara Santavicca

College of Health, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, USA

Rochelle L. Frounfelker

Department of Sociology, Université de Québec à Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada

Aoudou Njingouo Mounchingam

Division of Social and Cultural Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada

Cécile Rousseau

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Author DM contributed to conception and design of the study, interpretation of study findings, and writing the manuscript. Authors TS, RLF and ANM contributed to data analysis and drafting parts of the manuscript. Author CR contributed to the interpretation of study findings and provided feedback on several versions of the manuscript. The authors listed in the byline have agreed to the byline order and to submission of the manuscript in this form. All authors agreed to act as guarantor of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Diana Miconi .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Informed consent to participate was obtained from all of the participants in the study. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Educational and Psychological Institutional Review Board of the University of Montreal.

Consent for publication

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Miconi, D., Santavicca, T., Frounfelker, R.L. et al. Digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for violent radicalization among young Canadians: a latent profile analysis. BMC Psychol 12 , 260 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01739-0

Download citation

Received : 24 October 2023

Accepted : 18 April 2024

Published : 10 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01739-0

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Violent radicalization
  • Digital media use
  • Young people
  • Person-centered approach

BMC Psychology

ISSN: 2050-7283

research article social psychology

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Psychol Res Behav Manag

TikTok Tourette’s: Are We Witnessing a Rise in Functional Tic-Like Behavior Driven by Adolescent Social Media Use?

Jessica frey.

1 Department of Neurology, Rockefeller Neuroscience Institute, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA

Kevin J Black

2 Departments of Psychiatry, Neurology, Radiology, and Neuroscience, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA

Irene A Malaty

3 Department of Neurology, Fixel Institute for Neurological Diseases, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA

Perceptions of Tourette syndrome (TS) and tic disorders are often driven by social media. During the COVID-19 pandemic, social media consumption greatly increased, particularly in the adolescent population. In parallel with increased social media consumption, there has also been an increase in tic severity and functional tic-like behavior (FTLB). Given that many of the tic videos posted on social media are misleading, perpetuate false beliefs about TS, or reinforce tic-like behaviors, there is increasing concern that these videos are driving the rapid increase in FTLBs. Several studies have reviewed newly presenting cases of FTLB and have found shared characteristics, including that a higher proportion of affected individuals are female, there is a low proportion with a history of childhood or family tics, and symptom onset is typically acute and develops in the teenage years. In addition, the quality of the tics seen in association with FTLB mirrors many of the tics seen on popular social media channels, with higher rates of coprophenomena, tic attacks, and involvement of the trunk and extremities than is seen with typical tics. FTLBs are likely a specific subgroup of functional tics largely influenced by the portrayal of and growing popularity of functional tics posted on social media during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, several factors, including increased anxiety, social isolation, and social media use in general during the pandemic are likely also contributing factors to the surge of FTLBs seen recently. In this era of increased social media consumption, it will become increasingly important for clinicians to educate patients about where and how medical information is spread, to ensure the best possible diagnosis, treatment, and outcomes for patients.

Introduction

Tourette’s syndrome (TS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by multiple motor tics and at least one phonic tic, typically preceded by a premonitory urge. 1 , 2 To fulfill DSM-5 criteria, these symptoms must be present for at least one year and must be present before the age of 18. 2 TS is more common in males compared to females (~4:1), and most commonly begins during childhood around ages 4–8. 3 Chronic tic disorder (CTD) is diagnosed when an individual has either motor or phonic tics but not both. 1 There is often a decrease in tic frequency starting around the ages of 12–17, with 75% of children having considerable tic improvement during adulthood and up to 32% having complete tic remission. 4 , 5 However, although tics may improve during puberty, adolescents report this period of time as the most challenging from a social perspective since exclusion amongst peers due to tic symptoms often becomes more prominent. 6 In addition to tics, TS is associated with comorbid symptoms such as obsessive-compulsive behavior (OCB), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and mood disorders. 7

Quality of life can be severely impaired in patients with TS due to tics, including difficulties with family relationships in 29%, difficulties in making friends in 27%, difficulties with socialization in 20%, and feeling self-conscious of tics in 15%. 8 Beyond tics, up to 70% of individuals with TS reported non-tic-related impairments in quality of life, including difficulties in the school, home, or social environment. 9 School difficulties include increased likelihood of having an individualized education plan (IEP), increased likelihood of needing to repeat a grade, and higher rates of absenteeism. 10 , 11 One study of tic disorder impact involving 623 parents of children and 281 adults found that 35% of children and 77% of adults reported that tics interfered with achieving education commensurate with intellectual ability, and almost 20% of adult respondents reported subsequent limitation to their career paths as a result. 12 These barriers result from negative stigmas surrounding TS, often due to misperceptions of TS in the larger community by teachers, peers, and parents. 8 These stigmas can in turn lead to bullying and discrimination, with up to 75% of parents reporting that their child was treated differently due to their tics. 11 In addition, societal stigmas often lead to self-stigmatization, along with higher rates of depression and anxiety compared to the general population. 8 , 13 Many studies conclude that increasing education and awareness about what TS is and how it impacts an individual’s life can improve the overall well-being and quality of life for that individual. 8 , 10 , 13 For example, a few studies evaluating the knowledge of teachers and children about TS revealed only a basic knowledge that was amenable to educational interventions. 14 , 15

Since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, there has been a significant rise in sudden-onset, complex, and significantly disruptive tics with atypical features (described later). These presentations have been more consistent with functional neurological symptoms, which are thought to comprise a separate disorder, with characteristics distinct from typical organic tics. Sometimes referred to as functional tic-like behavior (FTLB), these tics can be seen in youth with no prior history of tics, and can also be seen to complicate the course of some youth with known tics and TS. This global phenomenon has resulted in concern that in the context of isolation and stress, attention and exposure to inaccurate portrayals of tics on social media may be fueling this notable rise. 16 , 17 There are also concerns that these social media platforms may unintentionally increase misperceptions of TS, furthering stigma and the sequelae of living with a stigmatized disorder. 16 , 18 Therefore, it is important not only to educate the general public about TS but also to educate individuals presenting with functional tics thought to be largely influenced by social media in order to reduce stigma, improve quality of life for those who have TS, and improve outcomes for individuals with functional tics. 3

This review will evaluate the complex relationship between social media, COVID-19, and the impact each of these has on tic-related disorders.

Functional Tics

Functional tics fall under the larger umbrella of functional movement disorders (FMDs), generally thought to be an external manifestation or reaction to underlying psychosocial stressors. 19 Functional tics have previously been referred to as pseudo-tics, psychogenic tics, and non-organic tics; however, the etiology of these non-typical tics has still not been well established and therefore the term functional tics is most often used as convention in recent years. 20

The phenomenological characteristics of tics in TS are somewhere between volitional movement and involuntary movement, 21 which makes distinguishing between typical tics of TS and functional tics challenging. There have been several proposals on how best to differentiate between typical tics and FTLBs, mainly based on clinical observations and case series of patients with atypical tics. Clues to the diagnosis of functional tics include a later onset in adolescence or young adulthood as opposed to childhood, sudden onset, complex tics at onset, and association with other functional neurologic disorders such as pseudoseizures. They occur more often in females as opposed to males, with a lack of childhood or family history of tics. 17 , 20–22 In addition, functional tics may be associated with a lack of premonitory sensation, unusual premonitory symptoms, a perception of being totally involuntary, poor response to typical tic medications, difficulty with suppression, and higher rates of suggestibility. 17 , 20–22 Certain behaviors are more common with functional tics, such as blocking tics, speaking in accents, as well as echo-, pali-, and coprophenomena. 3 , 23 The characteristics of the tics themselves may also have features distinctive of organic tics, such as occurring more often in the trunk and extremities as opposed to the head, face, and neck. 24 , 25 In addition, functional coprolalia is marked by longer and more complex phrases as well as a higher number of obscene words per individual. 21 The tics themselves may be more self-injurious or functionally limiting than is typically seen in TS. 3 , 23 However, shared risk factors such as anxiety and impulsivity, as well as overlap of clinical features can make it difficult to tell typical and functional tics apart. 23 This is further complicated by the fact that individuals with a typical history of TS can later develop functional tics that may co-occur with their previous tics. 20 , 21 , 23 , 26

There may be some overlap between the pathophysiology of typical tics and functional tics. In general, the pathophysiology of typical tics has clear influence of genetic determinants and is thought to be driven by a combination of two mechanisms, including enhanced reinforcement of motor learning via abnormally enhanced dopamine pathways and disinhibition of the cortico-thalamo-cortical loops leading to motor overflow. 23 Similar neural pathways have been postulated in association with functional neurologic disorders, where enhanced limbic-cortical activity may lead to reinforcement of abnormal motor patterns of movements and eventually lead to habituation of movement. 23 Functional tics may additionally be modified by attention to tics, perception of whether or not movements are voluntary, skewed sense of agency over movements, and behavioral reinforcement. 23 These modifiers, especially behavioral reinforcement, have become particularly relevant in the context of FTLBs seen recently. Observation of tics online via social media may lead to learning of this tic-like behavior and eventual habituation, which is discussed in more detail below.

Functional tics have been reported previously as a reaction to psychosocial stressors. 27 , 28 However, functional tics were previously reported as relatively rare compared to other functional movement disorders (2%) 20 and also rare compared to organic tics (1.9%). 29 , 30 Explosive or acute onset of tics was previously reported to represent only 5–8% of cases of TS. 24 , 31 Within the last several years, there has been a tremendous increase in acute-onset tics, tic attacks, and FTLBs, 24 , 25 , 30 , 32 with at least one study reporting a tenfold increase in the prevalence of FTLBs. 33 Although the rise of functional tics has been seen in a variety of distinctive geographic regions, the quality of the functional tics is similar and often exactly mimics the same symptoms posted online, leading many researchers to posit that the common thread between these individuals is the portrayal of functional tics in social media. 26

Social Media and COVID-19

In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, quarantining and physical social distancing quickly became strategies to try to prevent further spread of the disease. In so doing, many individuals turned to social media to continue to feel connected to the rest of society and to cope with increasing anxiety, fear, and uncertainty for the future. One study of over 2000 adolescents found that social media was overall a constructive coping strategy during the pandemic, leading to increased happiness in individuals with anxiety and loneliness. 34 While older children and adolescents used social media as a tool to become more informed about the pandemic, younger children often used social media to escape from the negativity associated with the pandemic and to seek out emotional support from others with an online presence. 34 , 35 As reliance on and interest in using social media has grown during the pandemic, there has also been a corresponding increase in excessive social media use, leading to addictive tendencies in some cases. 36 Interestingly, particular social media platforms were more likely to be associated with more addictive behaviors. For example, TikTok was found to be the most addictive social media platform amongst adolescents during the pandemic. 36 This finding correlates well with the fact that TikTok also had a tremendous increase in use during the pandemic. 24

Social media can be a valuable tool for disseminating information quickly, especially during a pandemic when time to publication could delay important information from becoming available to the public. 37 It may also allow for a sense of connectedness in otherwise isolated individuals. However, social media is a double-edged sword in that it can cause harm either directly by spreading misleading or false claims or indirectly by overwhelming social media posts with misinformation and making accurate content more difficult to find or engage with. 37 Misinformation spread via social media is not specific to TS or tics. In fact, several studies have demonstrated that information regarding COVID-19 on YouTube and TikTok is generally not useful or actually misleading. 38–40 Another study evaluated information accuracy regarding ADHD, a common comorbid symptom of TS, and found that 52% of the TikTok videos about ADHD were misleading. 41 Similar rates of misinformation have been reported for other medical conditions portrayed in social media such as acne and diabetes. 41 Social media therefore could potentially exert a detrimental influence on healthcare outcomes, as misinformation may lead to increased healthcare anxiety surrounding various conditions or inappropriate utilization of healthcare resources, based on representations that are viewed. 37 , 41

Portrayal of Tics in Media

Even before social media was a commonly used platform, tics and TS have been represented to the public in a variety of capacities, often through television, film, and literature. Many of these portrayals include exaggerated stereotypes and even incorrect depictions of individuals with TS. For example, characters with TS are often used as comic relief by demonstrating swearing tics or using profanity, even though the Tourette Association of America has concluded that only a small minority of patients experience coprolalia. 1 Studies have reported rates of coprolalia ranging from 10% to 30% and indicate that having coprolalia leads to a much poorer quality of life as opposed to the comedic representation of coprolalia seen in the media. 42–44 In addition, characters that were intended to have TS were incorrectly represented as having autism spectrum disorder instead, and interpersonal relationships with friends and family were often negatively portrayed, with increased conflict, difficulty communicating, and toxic relationship tendencies. 45 Other studies have also demonstrated that although some depictions can be realistic, the vast majority of depictions of tics are stereotyped portrayals, which inadvertently worsen the stigmatization of individuals with TS. 42 , 45–47 One study evaluating adolescents’ perceptions of peers with TS found that individuals with TS were thought to be deprived of agency and in need of assistance, leading to a “benevolence stigma” in combination with a reluctance to initiate any close or meaningful relationships with these individuals, due to fears of “social contamination.” 42 This study went on to conclude that the reason for these misperceptions primarily stemmed from misrepresentations of the condition in the media, which may be the only baseline exposure non-affected individuals are aware of from which to derive their understanding of TS. 42 These misrepresentations can also be seen on the internet. For example, online support groups can be an invaluable source of information and acceptance for individuals with TS, reducing social isolation and enabling accessibility when face-to-face resources may not be available or appropriate. 48 However, even though these support groups typically have good intentions, users have identified tic triggers, tic suggestibility, and conflict among other users of the online support groups as negatively affecting their experience and worsening their tics. 48 In some online forums, participants are encouraged to rate how severe their individual tics are and to compare to one another, leading to another layer of complexity in the psychodynamics of promoting illness or wellness. Finally, excessive engagement with tic-related content may lead to excessive focus on tics and detract from recognizing the individual as a whole person or other aspects of an individual’s identity and connectedness.

Portrayal of Tics in Social Media

The skewed portrayal of tics in social media is not new. In fact, a study from 2012 analyzed TS portrayals on YouTube and found that although there were some accurate and positive portrayals of TS available to view, negative portrayals of TS were associated with significantly more views. 47 This bias may inadvertently reinforce negative stereotypes and spread misinformation. 47 As early as 2016, there were documented cases of “tic attacks”, which were thought to represent a combination of tics and non-suppressible, disabling functional movements that could last for minutes to hours and often appeared to mimic seizure-like activity. 31 The authors concluded that these tic attacks likely stem from both internal and external contextual factors, including increased attentional focus on tics, a cognitive-affective feedback loop leading to increasing anxiety, and greater interoceptive awareness. 31

During the pandemic, there was an increased interest in TS via social media, leading to “#tourette” being viewed nearly 5 billion times on the TikTok platform alone. 3 , 49 Social media can, in fact, be a positive support system in which users have acknowledged that social media portrayals of tics have led to peer support and a sense of belonging. 24 However, there may be unintended consequences of viewing inaccurate portrayals of tics on social media, including reinforcement of maladaptive behaviors. Unfortunately, some of the misinformation propagated via social media may be financially motivated. For example, TikTok tic influencers sell merchandise related to the content of their videos or may even be paid to make an appearance. The videos with more views tend to be the videos portraying the most frequent, extraordinary, and violent tics, even when these are inaccurate portrayals. 24 These same popular videos are the ones that are most likely to generate the most income for the uploader. 24

Studies have sought to determine if there is a difference in phenomenology between organic and functional tics by analyzing the most-viewed videos on various social media platforms involving tics and TS. 17 , 24 Among 28 videos of TikTok influencers with a keyword of Tourette or tic, the average age was 19, 64% were female, 64% had tic attacks, 93% had coprolalia, and 86% had injurious behavior, features that are not consistent with the typical individual experiencing TS. 24 Similarly, another study also found higher rates of coprophenomena, self-injurious behavior, and environmental influences in videos labeled as “Tourette” on social media platforms such as TikTok, but that were believed to portray FTLB. 17 There are concerns from experts in the TS community that inaccurate and popular portrayals of tics such as these may actually increase stigmatization and marginalization of individuals with tics. 33 , 50

Social Media and COVID-19: Influence on Tic-Related Disorders

The COVID-19 pandemic clearly had an impact on tics, but this impact is complex and more nuanced than was originally thought. This complex influence will be analyzed for a variety of tic-related disorders including FMDs, PANDAS/PANS, and both typical and functional tics below.

Functional Movement Disorders (FMDs)

Since the onset of COVID-19, there has been growing interest in the number of functional movement disorders that have been reported across multiple institutions. Although there was no significant worsening of symptoms in patients who have preexisting FMD, there was an increase in the number of people who newly presented to clinic with FMD during the pandemic. 19 , 49 , 51 At least one study reported a 60.1% increase in new patients diagnosed with FMD between March 1, 2020 and October 30, 2020. 19 The majority of these new presentations occurred in female patients (75.6%), and the most common type of functional movement disorder reported was tremor (53.3%) followed by dystonia (31.1%). 19 Although functional tics have garnered quite the presence in news and media outlets, this study reported that functional tics accounted for only 8.9% of the FMDs that presented during COVID-19. 19 In addition, the rate of new-onset FMD following infection with COVID-19 was low, reported to only be 3% in comparison to other movement disorders seen following infection with COVID-19. 52

Although the pathophysiology of FMDs is unclear, stress does seem to play a role as a precipitating factor. In addition, altered connectivity between the limbic and motor networks as well as decreased activation of the SMA may represent neurobiological differences in individuals who develop FMDs. 19 It is hypothesized that prior traumas or other psychosocial stressors reshape the connectivity of the brain to make the brain more susceptible to having an FMD. 19 During a time when psychosocial stressors are high secondary to COVID-19, these risk factors could at least in part account for the increase in FMDs being seen at movement disorder clinics. The treatment for FMD remains challenging, but a combination of psychoeducation and cognitive behavioral therapy may be helpful. 53 Avoiding inadvertent reinforcement of the movements and addressing comorbidities such as depression, anxiety, and insomnia can also benefit patients with FMD. 54

Tic Triggers in Other Disorders

Given the longstanding controversial consideration that rare cases of immune-triggered tics may follow certain types of infections, one study sought to characterize tic and other symptom changes during the COVID-19 pandemic in individuals with a prior diagnosis of Pediatric Autoimmune Neuropsychiatric Disorders Associated with Streptococcal Infections or Pediatric Acute-Onset Neuropsychiatric Syndrome (PANDAS/PANS). Of note, evidence in support of a link between streptococcal infection and acute tic onset or exacerbation has been equivocal, 3 , 55 , 56 and association with other non-streptococcal infections has also been postulated. Among 108 included individuals that previously met the diagnostic criteria for PANDAS/PANS, 57 71% reported an increase in overall symptoms during the pandemic lockdown. 58 Interestingly, tics were more commonly reported to increase (56%) than were symptoms from other domains, all of which were below 50%, although it is unclear why tics were reported to have increased more in comparison to other symptoms. 58 Environmental factors were suspected to account for worsening symptoms in these individuals, with sleep disruption proposed to be a key contributor. This same study identified the following risk factors for worsening neuropsychiatric symptoms: changes in parental work routine, parental stress or anxiety transmitted to children, boredom, being quarantined to a small living space, and inadequate information related to COVID-19. 58 These same risk factors have also been identified for symptom worsening in disorders other than PANDAS/PANS, including TS. 18 , 53 , 55 This highlights the importance of psychosocial environment in aggravating pre-existing symptoms.

Some individuals with tics may traverse the pandemic with no major changes in baseline tic severity, or even experience reduced tics during periods of less frequent social interactions. However, overall presentations to American emergency departments for tics increased substantially during the pandemic peak. 59 , 60 The pandemic has exerted a negative influence on tics in the following three manners: worsening of tics in individuals who already have a preexisting history of tics, the acute onset of tic attacks or FTLBs, or a combination of worsening functional and typical tics in a single individual. However, because phenomenological features of TS and FTLB can overlap, distinguishing the symptoms can be difficult. 17

Early in the pandemic, a study revealed that 48% of individuals with preexisting TS or CTD reported worsening of their tics and cited reasons such as increased anxiety due to fears surrounding the future, quarantining, fewer distractions, and changes in routine. 61 Similarly, there was a worsening of symptoms in 67% of adolescents during the beginning of the pandemic, including more severe tics, hyperactivity, rage attacks, obsessions/compulsions, and anxiety. 18

The COVID-19 pandemic was initially postulated to impact tic symptoms through a variety of avenues, including anxiety related to the pandemic, confinement/quarantine, alterations in tics specific to the pandemic, and neurotropic effects of COVID-19. 55 The neurotropic effects are now thought much less likely to contribute to tic symptoms, and indirect impacts of COVID-19 are more likely to modify tic symptoms. For example, fears related to the pandemic, changes in work or financial stability, poor health of loved ones, or restricted freedom due to quarantine may contribute to anxiety that can in turn lead to worsening of tics. Indeed, one study found that in parents of children with tics, 32% had a significant reduction in income and 17% experienced unexpected unemployment in both parents. 18 On the other hand, some individuals reported improvements in their tics because quarantining or virtual school removed many of the triggers that usually lead to worse tics. 18 Up to 87% of children with tics have reported participating in online learning at home during the pandemic, and aside from school activities, the most popular pastimes were playing video games (69%) and interacting on social media (54%). 18 Finally, pandemic-specific tics such as coughing or throat clearing may become more troubling or lead to worse stigmatization given public fears of contamination and spread of viral disease. 55

In addition to alterations in tic symptoms in individuals who already carry a diagnosis of TS or CTD, there have also been reports of increasing cases of functional tics. Given the rapid onset and similarity to many tics represented in social media, it was proposed that these functional tics may be an example of a mass sociogenic illness. 33 This is not the first time that tics have been “spread” by observing another individual’s behaviors. For example, there was an infamous outbreak of tic-like behavior in 19 students at Le Roy High School in 2012, sometimes referred to as “mass hysteria.” 33 Due to their spread via social media, these functional tics may be a new digital equivalent termed “mass social media-induced illness” (MSMI). 33 Some of the signs linking these functional tics to social media include the nearly identical replication of some tics (such as saying “beans”, or “woo hoo” or specific self-injurious or incapacitating tics similar to those seen online), 62 as well as adoption of similar behaviors such as giving a name to the tic symptoms. 33 , 50 Other indications that these functional tics may be a distinctive entity are that the tics tend to be severe enough to limit performing unpleasant tasks but abate when doing enjoyable activities, and remission of these tics was seen in some patients after a diagnosis of TS was excluded. 33 Of course, it is important to remain cautious in identifying any causative forces without clear supporting evidence. 63 Indeed, several studies have proposed that the functional tics seen in association with social media use are likely caused by a combination of complex factors, including predisposing factors such as pre-existing genetic risk factors for depression and anxiety, poor adaptive or coping behaviors, family and peer-related stressors, pandemic-specific impacts on mental health, social isolation, and behavioral modeling through exposure to social media. 16 , 30 , 62

Regardless, several case reports, case series, and cohort studies have collectively identified over 100 patients that have been newly presented to a variety of institutions with what is believed to be FTLB ( Table 1 ). Although the causative effect of social media has not been definitively linked to the onset of FTLBs, it is hard to ignore the similarities in pattern and behavior between the social media influencers and the patients presenting to movement disorder clinics around the world.

Summary of Studies Evaluating FTLB Patients, Including Demographic Information, Exposure to Social Media Portrayals of Tics Prior to Onset, Tic Characteristics, and Comorbid Features

Those presenting with FTLB share similar characteristics across reports: tic onset tends to be sudden and in teenage years, the majority are female, and few have a history of childhood or family tics. Tic characteristics are also strikingly similar: acute onset of complex movements predominately involving the trunk and extremities rather than the more usual tic onset with simple tics evolving over time to more complex and without the usual rostro-caudal evolution. In addition, there were high percentages of self-injurious behavior, coprophenomena, tic attacks, comorbid FMDs, and comorbid depression or anxiety. 25 , 30 , 32 , 62 , 64–66 A high percentage of patients with FTLB were first seen in the emergency department due to the acuity and severity of their initial presentation. 64 While there are several features that distinguish FTLBs from typical tics, there are also features that may or may not be present in either manifestation of tics, and may not necessarily help to differentiate between them. Specifically, suppressibility, premonitory urges, and comorbid symptoms such as anxiety could be seen in either population. 25 , 62 , 64 , 65 In general, there were higher rates of anxiety and depression, lower rates of ADHD and OCD, and a lack of agency over movements was more often reported in patients with FTLBs compared to patients with typical tics. 30 , 62 , 65 The demographic and clinical features of these patients are consistent with the videos posted in social media portraying FTLB. 17 , 24 In addition, these features are also consistent with prior descriptions that have sought to differentiate functional tics from TS. 20–22 Finally, many patients reported watching social media tic-related videos prior to their presentation to a movement disorder clinic with FTLB, although duration of exposure to this social media content was not documented in most studies. 25 , 30 , 32 , 62 , 64 , 65

A 6-month follow-up of the patients with FTLB in the studies by Howlett et al found that there was an average decrease in the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS) of 31.9 and 19.6 points in adolescents and adults, respectively, which suggests that adolescents may have a better prognosis than adults, or at least respond more quickly. 67 The most effective treatment approaches for achieving reduction in functional tic symptoms are still being investigated. To date, there are no randomized controlled studies dedicated to treatments specifically for functional tics. Some studies observed improvement in functional tics when treating underlying comorbidities such as anxiety and depression. 67 In general, pharmacologic therapies used for the treatment of typical tics (ie alpha-2-adrenergic agonists, antipsychotics, etc.) have not been as effective for functional tics, and lack of response to pharmacotherapy may actually be an indicator of functional disease, 23 although formal studies assessing the efficacy of pharmacologic therapy for functional tics have not yet been performed. Motor retraining aiming to reestablish normal movement patterns through physical and occupational therapy may be helpful for functional tics. 24 Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is a type of behavior modification that has been effectively used to improve symptoms of other functional neurologic disorders 68 and may therefore also be helpful for functional tics. CBT works by teaching patients competing or distracting maneuvers to refocus attention away from the abnormal movements. 23 , 68 An alternative behavioral modification approach, called comprehensive behavioral intervention for tics (CBIT), combines habit reversal therapy, relaxation training, and psychoeducation to help identify tic triggers or reinforcers and to redirect abnormal movements into a competing, voluntary action. CBIT is an efficacious treatment for typical tics, and while there may be some role for CBIT in the treatment of functional tics, studies have not yet directly assessed CBIT for functional tics. In addition, since exposure to functional tics via social media may lead to inadvertent behavioral reinforcement of such tics, exposure reduction should be considered by limiting social media with misleading content. Educating the patient and family about appropriate, valid, and reliable resources is one of the first steps to reducing exposure to misleading social media content. Other mechanisms of reducing exposure to misinformation have been proposed including redesigning social media search algorithms so that credible sources of information are elevated to the top, tagging content that is evidenced based versus not evidence based, using systematic surveillance to detect spread of misinformation, and training clinicians how to help patients identify reliable sources of information. 69 Finally, educating the patient and family members about functional tics is extremely important for fostering acceptance of the diagnosis and working toward symptom and quality of life improvement. 62 Similarly, educating the public about functional diagnoses such as FTLB will help to reduce the stigma that comes with both FMDs and TS.

To summarize, the FTLBs that have been observed during the pandemic may be a distinct subtype of functional tics strongly influenced by portrayals in social media. Tic characteristics such as higher rates of coprophenomena, predominant arm and trunk involvement, intense and potentially self-injurious movements, and complex vocalizations along with distinctly later onset may flag the need to consider FTLB. A greater representation of females among those affected, association with other functional neurologic disorders, and lack of prior history of childhood tics can be helpful in distinguishing FTLB, but must be considered together with clinical features and overall clinical presentation. Importantly, clinicians should remain diagnostically cautious since FTLB and organic tics can coexist in a single individual.

While there are many factors that have likely contributed to the increase in FTLB seen during the pandemic, there is no question that social media is one of these factors given the rapid rise across geographic and cultural borders and striking similarities between the tics on social media and the FTLB seen in clinic. 30 , 49 , 62 , 64 , 66 In a somewhat ironic cycle, the influence of social media on tics has gained popularity in news and media outlets, spreading the popularity of these videos to even wider audiences. However, the relationship between these social media posts and the onset of new FTLB is much more complex than a straightforward cause-and-effect relationship. Other factors such as social isolation during the pandemic, increased rates of social media use during the pandemic, increased rates of depression and anxiety, and instability as a direct or indirect result of the pandemic also likely contribute to the rising rates of FTLB that have been documented during the pandemic.

Given this era of increased social media consumption and reliance on virtual technology to communicate remotely, the rise of FTLB is unlikely to be the only mass sociogenic illness spread. Indeed, it will be important for clinicians to remain vigilant about where and how medical information is consumed and to properly educate patients regarding medical diagnoses and treatments. Recognizing FTLB as a separate entity is important for many reasons, including treatment for the functional symptoms, avoiding unnecessary hospitalizations, minimizing unnecessary diagnostic tests, and allocating appropriate resources and treatments for patients. The more that clinicians are able to identify risk factors for and make accurate diagnoses of FTLB, the more that patients will be able to receive proper treatment and care and ultimately achieve a better quality of life.

Dr Jessica Frey reports grants from the Tourette Association of America, outside the submitted work. Dr Kevin J Black reports personal fees from SK Life Science, Inc., Medscape, Mededicus, and Huntington Study Group; grants from Emalex Biosciences, outside the submitted work; and he is also an invited member of a working group on functional tic-like behavior convened by the Tourette Association of America. Dr Irene A Malaty participated in research funded by Teva/Nuvelution, outside the submitted work; and she serves on the medical advisory board of the Tourette Association of America (uncompensated). The authors report no other conflicts of interest in this work.

IMAGES

  1. 15 Social Psychology Examples (2024)

    research article social psychology

  2. (PDF) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    research article social psychology

  3. An Introduction To Social Psychology

    research article social psychology

  4. PPT

    research article social psychology

  5. (PDF) Why the New Review of Social Psychology?

    research article social psychology

  6. Social Psychology

    research article social psychology

VIDEO

  1. PSY 2510 Social Psychology: What is social psychology?

  2. what is social psychology: meaning, nature, scope, importance

  3. Intro to Social Psychology (Intro Psych Tutorial #186)

  4. What is Social Psychology? An Introduction

  5. Research Methods in Psychology: Types of Psychology Studies

  6. Sociology Research Methods: Crash Course Sociology #4

COMMENTS

  1. The Journal of Social Psychology

    Since John Dewey and Carl Murchison founded it in 1929, The Journal of Social Psychology has published original empirical research in all areas of basic and applied social psychology. Most articles report laboratory or field research in core areas of social and organizational psychology including the self and social identity, person perception and social cognition, attitudes and persuasion ...

  2. Social Psychological Theory as History: Outlining the Critical

    I will refer to it as the "mainstream epistemology" of social psychology, characterizing especially research conducted with convenience samples in WEIRD settings (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010), published in the major journals of the discipline (e.g., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology), and rooted in the "canon" of U.S ...

  3. Social Psychology Quarterly: Sage Journals

    Social Psychology Quarterly. Social Psychology Quarterly (SPQ) publishes theoretical and empirical papers on the link between the individual and society. This includes the study of the relations of individuals to one another, as well as to groups, collectivities, and institutions. It also … | View full journal description.

  4. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ® publishes original papers in all areas of personality and social psychology and emphasizes empirical reports, but may include specialized theoretical, methodological, and review papers.. The journal is divided into three independently edited sections. Attitudes and Social Cognition publishes articles concerning attitudinal and social cognitive ...

  5. Frontiers in Social Psychology

    Sandra C. Matz. Frontiers in Social Psychology. doi 10.3389/frsps.2023.1290295. 1,444 views. A broad-scope, multidisciplinary journal covering the spectrum of social psychology, exploring how human behavior and development is affected by social interactions.

  6. Frontiers in Social Psychology

    A broad-scope, multidisciplinary journal covering the spectrum of social psychology, exploring how human behavior and development is affected by social interactions . ... Submit your research. Start your submission and get more impact for your research by publishing with us. Author guidelines.

  7. International Journal of Social Psychology: Sage Journals

    Revista de Psicología Social publishes original works of empirical research, and exceptionally theoretical reviews, from all fields of Social Psychology. Revista de Psicología Social will be of interest to researchers and professionals of Social Psychology, … | View full journal description. This journal is a member of the Committee on ...

  8. Social Psychology: Articles, Research, & Case Studies on Social

    People who seem like they're paying attention often aren't—even when they're smiling and nodding toward the speaker. Research by Alison Wood Brooks, Hanne Collins, and colleagues reveals just how prone the mind is to wandering, and sheds light on ways to stay tuned in to the conversation. 1. 2.

  9. Social Psychology and Social Processes

    Workplace racial composition is an important factor for Black parents' communication of racial socialization messages. from Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. November 28, 2023. Children's ethnic-racial identity and mothers' cultural socialization are protective for young Latine children's mental health.

  10. 182746 PDFs

    Social Psychology | Explore the latest full-text research PDFs, articles, conference papers, preprints and more on SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY. Find methods information, sources, references or conduct a ...

  11. Social and Personality Psychology Compass

    1. INTRODUCTION. Humans are inherently social, with a fundamental need to belong and forge meaningful connections with others (Baumeister & Leary, 1995).Indeed, social connection is critical to individuals' well‐being, and deficits in social connection can have devastating consequences, including increased risk for mortality that persists even after controlling for comorbidities (Cacioppo ...

  12. <em>British Journal of Social Psychology</em>

    British Journal of Social Psychology is an international journal publishing impactful basic and applied social psychological research from all parts of the world. Abstract Natural resources are limited, and people often share these limited resources in groups, which creates an intergroup resource dilemma.

  13. Current Research in Social Psychology

    CRISP is permanently archived at the Library of the University of Iowa and at the Library of Congress. Beginning in April, 2000, Sociological Abstracts publishes the abstracts of CRISP articles. Citation Format: Lastname, Firstname. 1996. "Title of Article." Current Research in Social Psychology 2:15-22 https://crisp.org.uiowa.edu

  14. Social Psychology

    Social Psychology publishes innovative and methodologically sound research and serves as an international forum for scientific discussion and debate in the field of social psychology. Topics include all basic social psychological research themes, methodological advances in social psychology, as well as research in applied fields of social psychology.

  15. Social psychology: Predicting other people's attention to understand

    Communications Psychology - Humans are highly social beings who are interested in what others are saying, thinking, and doing. ... Research articles Reviews & Analysis News & Comment ...

  16. Social Psychology

    Social Psychology of Violence. Daniel Christie, Michael Wessells, in Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, & Conflict (Second Edition), 2008. Social psychology emphasizes situational and cognitive influences on violence and its prevention. Using a systems approach, this article examines the causes of both direct and structural violence at levels ranging from the interpersonal to the societal and ...

  17. Social psychology as a natural kind

    Box 1The self in social psychology. Research on the self encompasses a variety of phenomena, from self-referential thought and self-initiated behavior to self-regulation and self-esteem. Although there is no single definition of the self, social psychology has paid special attention to one particular aspect of selfhood: one's concept of self ...

  18. Social Psychology Quarterly

    Corrigendum to "Charlemagne's Legacy: A Consensus Analysis of Affective Meanings in French and German Culture". Free access Correction First published December 8, 2023 pp. 106. xml PDF / EPUB. Table of contents for Social Psychology Quarterly, 87, 1, Mar 01, 2024.

  19. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    The Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (JESP) aims to publish articles that extend or create conceptual advances in social psychology. As the title of the journal indicates, we are focused on publishing primary reports of research in social psychology that use experimental or quasi-experimental methods, although not every study in an article needs to be experimental.

  20. The 9 Major Research Areas in Social Psychology

    Attitudes. Violence and Aggression. Prosocial Behavior. Prejudice and Discrimination. Social Identity. Group Behavior. Social Influence. Interpersonal Relationships. Social psychology is a branch of psychology that studies a wide range of subjects related to social behavior.

  21. Mechanisms linking social media use to adolescent mental ...

    Research linking social media use and adolescent mental health has produced mixed and inconsistent findings and little translational evidence, despite pressure to deliver concrete recommendations ...

  22. Journals Related to Social Psychology

    This page contains journals related to social psychology, personality psychology, and general psychology. For additional journal information, see: ... Current Research in Social Psychology (electronic journal) European Journal of Social Psychology (EASP-sponsored journal) European Review of Social Psychology (an e-first journal)

  23. The moderating role of psychological power distance on the ...

    Destructive leadership, a prevalent negative behavior in modern organizations, continues to captivate the interest of scholars and professionals due to its detrimental aftermath. Drawing from social psychological (culture) and conservation of resources theory, we explore the moderating impact of psychological power distance on the link between destructive leadership and emotional exhaustion ...

  24. New Content From Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological

    Despite its popularity, the replication crisis in social psychology and other fields, as well as methodological limitations associated with the paradigm, raise concerns about the robustness of classic studies in this literature. ... The use of linear mixed models (LMMs) is increasing in psychology and neuroscience research In this article, we ...

  25. Frontiers

    The effects of word frequency in turns-at-talk, the nucleus of social action in conversation, have, by contrast, been largely neglected. This study probes into this gap by applying corpus-linguistic methods on the conversational component of the British National Corpus (BNC) and the Freiburg Multimodal Interaction Corpus (FreMIC).

  26. Digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for violent

    The recent increase in support for - and direct engagement in - ideologically motivated violence among youth can be associated with the increase in social polarization in society [] as well as the specificities of adolescence and early adulthood, a seminal period for the development of ideologies [2, 3].Violent radicalization (VR) is a complex and multidimensional phenomenon [] defined as ...

  27. TikTok Tourette's: Are We Witnessing a Rise in Functional Tic-Like

    Introduction. Tourette's syndrome (TS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by multiple motor tics and at least one phonic tic, typically preceded by a premonitory urge. 1, 2 To fulfill DSM-5 criteria, these symptoms must be present for at least one year and must be present before the age of 18. 2 TS is more common in males compared to females (~4:1), and most commonly begins ...

  28. Americans share fake news to fit in with social circles

    Washington — Both conservative and liberal Americans share fake news because they don't want to be ostracized from their social circles, according to research published by the American Psychological Association. "Conformity and social pressure are key motivators of the spread of fake news," said lead researcher Matthew Asher Lawson, PhD, an assistant professor of decision sciences at ...